Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 8 |
Research Methodology | 8 |
Responses | 8 |
Psychological Studies | 5 |
Flow Charts | 4 |
Tables (Data) | 3 |
Experiments | 2 |
Heart Rate | 2 |
Perception | 2 |
Task Performance | 2 |
Auditory Perception | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 8 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Frings, Christian; Wentura, Dirk – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
It is an accepted, albeit puzzling finding that negative priming (NP) hinges on the presence of distractors in probe displays. In three experiments without probe distractors, the authors yielded evidence that response-biasing processes based on the contingency between prime and probe displays may have caused this finding. It is argued that it is…
Descriptors: Experiments, Responses, Experimental Psychology, Statistical Bias

Beller, Henry K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975
In this article it is argued that reading is faster than naming because printed words are more compatible with verbal responses than are objects. (Editor)
Descriptors: Diagrams, Experimental Psychology, Psychological Studies, Reaction Time

Duda, Peter D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975
The purpose of this study was to establish a theoretical framework for Stevens' empirically derived power law. Three models were proposed to explain the power law. (Editor)
Descriptors: Data Analysis, Experimental Psychology, Models, Psychological Studies

Kubovy, Michael; Psotka, Joseph – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976
When asked to report the first digit that comes to mind, a predominant number (28.4 percent) of the respondents choose 7. Three further experiments sought to establish whether this predominance is due to an automatic activation process or to a deliberate choice. (Editor)
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Experiments, Flow Charts, Numbers

Graham, Frances K.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975
This article reports the first of a series of studies to determine whether the lead-stimulus paradigm would yield results in man similar to those obtained in other organisms. (Author)
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Eye Movements, Flow Charts, Heart Rate

Wardlaw, Kirk A.; Kroll, Neal E. A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976
Names of cities were shock associated and then embedded in material presented to the nonattended channel in a dichotic listening and shadowing situation. A test for conditioning followed the dichotic listening task. Comparisons are made with other studies of galvanic skin responses to unattended words. (Editor/RK)
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Conditioning, Experimental Psychology, Flow Charts

Coles, Michael G. H.; Duncan-Johnson, Connie C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Flow Charts, Heart Rate, Information Processing

Levy, Jerre; Trevarthen, Colwyn – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976
Four commissurotomy patients were tested for ability to match tachistoscopically presented stimuli with pictures in free vision, according to either structural appearance or functional/conceptual category. (Editor)
Descriptors: Cerebral Dominance, Charts, Cognitive Processes, Experimental Psychology