NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gahl, Susanne; Yao, Yao; Johnson, Keith – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Frequent or contextually predictable words are often phonetically reduced, i.e. shortened and produced with articulatory undershoot. Explanations for phonetic reduction of predictable forms tend to take one of two approaches: Intelligibility-based accounts hold that talkers maximize intelligibility of words that might otherwise be difficult to…
Descriptors: Speech, Phonetics, Language Acquisition, Vowels
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cai, Zhenguang G.; Pickering, Martin J.; Yan, Hao; Branigan, Holly P. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Bilinguals appear to have shared syntactic representations for similar constructions between languages but retain distinct representations for noncognate translation-equivalents (Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2007). We inquire whether bilinguals have more integrated representations of cognate translation-equivalents. To investigate…
Descriptors: Syntax, Second Language Learning, Sentences, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jill R. Hoover; Holly L. Storkel; Tiffany P. Hogan – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Two experiments examined the effects of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word learning by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Nonwords orthogonally varying in probability and density were taught with learning and retention measured via picture naming. Experiment 1 used a within story probability/across story density exposure…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Probability, Language Acquisition, Case Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
White, Katherine S.; Morgan, James L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
In previous studies of phonological sensitivity, toddlers have failed to differentiate mispronunciations of varying severity. We provide evidence of more sophisticated phonological knowledge. Nineteen-month-olds were presented with displays consisting of one familiar and one unfamiliar object. In Experiment 1, names of familiar objects were…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Phonology, Language Acquisition, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Au, Terry Kit-fong; Oh, Janet S.; Knightly, Leah M.; Jun, Sun-Ah; Romo, Laura F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Childhood experience with a language seems to help adult learners speak it with a more native-like accent. Can analogous benefits be found beyond phonology? This study focused on adult learners of Spanish who had spoken Spanish as their native language before age 7 and only minimally, if at all, thereafter until they began to re-learn Spanish…
Descriptors: Phonology, Child Language, Native Speakers, Pronunciation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Merriman, William E.; Lipko, Amanda R. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Preschool-age children were hypothesized to use one of two criteria, cue recognition or target generation, to make several linguistic judgments. When deciding whether a word is one they know, for example, some were expected to consider whether they recognized its sound form (cue recognition), whereas others were expected to consider whether a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Metalinguistics, Semantics, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gout, A.; Christophe, A.; Morgan, J. L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
The location of phonological phrase boundaries was shown to affect lexical access by English-learning infants of 10 and 13 months of age. Experiments 1 and 2 used the head-turn preference procedure: infants were familiarized with two bisyllabic words, then presented with sentences that either contained the familiarized words or contained both…
Descriptors: Infants, Sentences, Syllables, Word Recognition