Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Language Processing | 10 |
Prediction | 10 |
Models | 6 |
Semantics | 3 |
Sentences | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
English | 2 |
Nouns | 2 |
Oral Language | 2 |
Regression (Statistics) | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 10 |
Author
Adelman, James S. | 1 |
Arnold, Jennifer E. | 1 |
Bell, Alan | 1 |
Brenier, Jason M. | 1 |
Brown, Gordon D. A. | 1 |
Burgess, Neil | 1 |
Federmeier, Kara D. | 1 |
Flude, Brenda | 1 |
Girand, Cynthia | 1 |
Glucksberg, Sam | 1 |
Gregory, Michelle | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Leslie, Sarah-Jane; Khemlani, Sangeet; Glucksberg, Sam – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Generics are statements such as "tigers are striped" and "ducks lay eggs". They express general, though not universal or exceptionless, claims about kinds (Carlson & Pelletier, 1995). For example, the generic "ducks lay eggs" seems true even though many ducks (e.g. the males) do not lay eggs. The universally quantified version of the statement…
Descriptors: Prediction, Cognitive Processes, Generalization, Language Processing
Kahn, Jason M.; Arnold, Jennifer E. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Givenness tends to lead to acoustic reduction in speech, but little is known about whether linguistic and non-linguistic givenness affect reduction similarly, and there is little consensus about the underlying psychological mechanisms. We examined speakers' pronunciations of target object nouns in an instruction-giving task, where speakers saw…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Speech Communication, Nouns, Language Processing
Szewczyk, Jakub M.; Schriefers, Herbert – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
Recently, several ERP studies have shown that the human language comprehension system anticipates words that are highly likely continuations of a given text. However, it remains an open issue whether the language comprehension system can also make predictions that go beyond a specific word. Here, we address the question of whether readers predict…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Language Processing, Prediction, Literary Genres
McKoon, Gail; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
In the domain of discourse processing, it has been claimed that older adults (60-0-year-olds) are less likely to encode and remember some kinds of information from texts than young adults. The experiment described here shows that they do make a particular kind of inference to the same extent that college-age adults do. The inferences examined were…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Theory Practice Relationship, Young Adults, Inferences
Laszlo, Sarah; Federmeier, Kara D. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Two related questions critical to understanding the predictive processes that come online during sentence comprehension are (1) what information is included in the representation created through prediction and (2) at what functional stage does top-down, predicted information begin to affect bottom-up word processing? We investigated these…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Prediction, Language Processing
Norris, Dennis; Kinoshita, Sachiko; van Casteren, Maarten – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Early on during word recognition, letter positions are not accurately coded. Evidence for this comes from transposed-letter (TL) priming effects, in which letter strings generated by transposing two adjacent letters (e.g., "jugde") produce large priming effects, more than primes with the letters replaced in the corresponding position (e.g.,…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Language Processing, Sampling, Coding
Hitch, Graham J.; Flude, Brenda; Burgess, Neil – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Three experiments tested predictions of a neural network model of phonological short-term memory that assumes separate representations for order and item information, order being coded via a context-timing signal [Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. "Psychological…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Models, Prediction, Experiments
Bell, Alan; Brenier, Jason M.; Gregory, Michelle; Girand, Cynthia; Jurafsky, Dan – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
In a regression study of conversational speech, we show that frequency, contextual predictability, and repetition have separate contributions to word duration, despite their substantial correlations. We also found that content- and function-word durations are affected differently by their frequency and predictability. Content words are shorter…
Descriptors: Oral Language, English, Prediction, Regression (Statistics)
Yang, Jianfeng; McCandliss, Bruce D.; Shu, Hua; Zevin, Jason D. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Many theoretical models of reading assume that different writing systems require different processing assumptions. For example, it is often claimed that print-to-sound mappings in Chinese are not represented or processed sub-lexically. We present a connectionist model that learns the print-to-sound mappings of Chinese characters using the same…
Descriptors: Test Items, Speech, Models, Oral Language
Adelman, James S.; Brown, Gordon D. A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Models of visual word recognition have been assessed by both factorial and regression approaches. Factorial approaches tend to provide a relatively weak test of models, and regression approaches give little indication of the sources of models' mispredictions, especially when parameters are not optimal. A new alternative method, involving…
Descriptors: Investigations, Word Recognition, Visual Stimuli, Models