Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Reaction Time | 3 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Language Processing | 2 |
Undergraduate Students | 2 |
Auditory Perception | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Classification | 1 |
Color | 1 |
Computer Uses in Education | 1 |
Difficulty Level | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Language and Cognitive… | 3 |
Author
Ashley, Aaron | 1 |
Carlson, Laura A. | 1 |
Cleland, Alexandra A. | 1 |
Gaskell, M. Gareth | 1 |
Hennessey, Neville W. | 1 |
Quinlan, Philip T. | 1 |
Tamminen, Jakke | 1 |
Truman, Amanda | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 3 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Cleland, Alexandra A.; Tamminen, Jakke; Quinlan, Philip T.; Gaskell, M. Gareth – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2012
We report 3 experiments that examined whether presentation of a spoken word creates an attentional bottleneck associated with lexical processing in the absence of a response to that word. A spoken word and a visual stimulus were presented in quick succession, but only the visual stimulus demanded a response. Response times to the visual stimulus…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Auditory Perception, Visual Perception, Language Processing
Ashley, Aaron; Carlson, Laura A. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2007
The location of an object is often described by spatially relating it to a known landmark. The spatial terms used in such descriptions can provide various types of information. For example, projective terms such as "above" indicate direction but not distance, whereas proximal terms such as "near" indicate distance but not direction. Previous…
Descriptors: Verbs, Spatial Ability, Language Skills, Classification
Truman, Amanda; Hennessey, Neville W. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2006
Twenty-four children with dyslexia (aged 7;7 to 12;1) and twenty-four age-matched controls named pictures aloud while hearing nonsense syllables either phonologically related (i.e., part of) or unrelated to the target picture name. Compared with controls, dyslexics had slower reaction times overall and, for low frequency items, the degree of…
Descriptors: Dyslexia, Children, Phonology, Cognitive Processes