Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Models | 6 |
Entomology | 3 |
Olfactory Perception | 3 |
Animals | 2 |
Associative Learning | 2 |
Brain | 2 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 2 |
Classical Conditioning | 2 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Memory | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Learning & Memory | 6 |
Author
Ayyub, Champakali | 1 |
Commins, Sean | 1 |
Cox, Brittney M. | 1 |
Das, Abhijit | 1 |
Devaud, Jean-Marc | 1 |
Diviney, Mairead | 1 |
Fey, Dirk | 1 |
Gall, Christine M. | 1 |
Giurfa, Martin | 1 |
Hourcade, Benoit | 1 |
Ito, Kei | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Quintanilla, Julian; Cox, Brittney M.; Gall, Christine M.; Mahler, Stephen V.; Lynch, Gary – Learning & Memory, 2021
Evidence suggests encoding of recent episodic experiences may be enhanced by a subsequent salient event. We tested this hypothesis by giving rats a 3-min unsupervised experience with four odors and measuring retention after different delays. Animals recognized that a novel element had been introduced to the odor set at 24 but not 48 h. However,…
Descriptors: Evidence, Memory, Animals, Olfactory Perception
Diviney, Mairead; Fey, Dirk; Commins, Sean – Learning & Memory, 2013
Learning to navigate toward a goal is an essential skill. Place learning is thought to rely on the ability of animals to associate the location of a goal with surrounding environmental cues. Using the Morris water maze, a task popularly used to examine place learning, we demonstrate that distal cues provide animals with distance and directional…
Descriptors: Cues, Learning Processes, Task Analysis, Animals
Larkin, Aoife; Karak, Somdatta; Priya, Rashi; Das, Abhijit; Ayyub, Champakali; Ito, Kei; Rodrigues, Veronica; Ramaswami, Mani – Learning & Memory, 2010
Naive "Drosophila" larvae show vigorous chemotaxis toward many odorants including ethyl acetate (EA). Chemotaxis toward EA is substantially reduced after a 5-min pre-exposure to the odorant and recovers with a half-time of [image omitted]20 min. An analogous behavioral decrement can be induced without odorant-receptor activation through…
Descriptors: Habituation, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Inhibition, Entomology
Hourcade, Benoit; Perisse, Emmanuel; Devaud, Jean-Marc; Sandoz, Jean-Christophe – Learning & Memory, 2009
The storage of stable memories is generally considered to rely on changes in the functional properties and/or the synaptic connectivity of neural networks. However, these changes are not easily tractable given the complexity of the learning procedures and brain circuits studied. Such a search can be narrowed down by studying memories of specific…
Descriptors: Classical Conditioning, Long Term Memory, Brain, Memory
Metherate, Raju – Learning & Memory, 2004
Acetylcholine release in sensory neocortex contributes to higher-order sensory function, in part by activating nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs). Molecular studies have revealed a bewildering array of nAChR subtypes and cellular actions; however, there is some consensus emerging about the major nAChR subtypes and their functions in…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Stimulation, Biochemistry, Neurology
Giurfa, Martin; Malun, Dagmar – Learning & Memory, 2004
The present work introduces a form of associative mechanosensory conditioning of the proboscis extension reflex (PER) in honeybees. In our paradigm, harnessed honeybees learn the elemental association between mechanosensory, antennal stimulation and a reward of sucrose solution delivered to the proboscis. Thereafter, bees extend their proboscis to…
Descriptors: Models, Cues, Stimulation, Classical Conditioning