Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Adolescents | 13 |
Child Development | 13 |
Visual Perception | 13 |
Children | 11 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Spatial Ability | 5 |
Cognitive Ability | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Developmental Stages | 4 |
Visual Stimuli | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Abdi, Herve | 1 |
Antezana, Ligia | 1 |
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Ardila, Alfredo | 1 |
Barclay, Allan G. | 1 |
Batty, Magali | 1 |
Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne | 1 |
Butler, Robert | 1 |
Caeyenberghs, Karen | 1 |
Damian, Markus F. | 1 |
Dumontheil, Iroise | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 10 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Colombia | 1 |
Mexico | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jerger, Susan; Damian, Markus F.; Tye-Murrey, Nancy; Abdi, Herve – Journal of Child Language, 2017
Adults use vision to perceive low-fidelity speech; yet how children acquire this ability is not well understood. The literature indicates that children show reduced sensitivity to visual speech from kindergarten to adolescence. We hypothesized that this pattern reflects the effects of complex tasks and a growth period with harder-to-utilize…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Visual Perception, Preschool Children, Children
Antezana, Ligia; Mosner, Maya G.; Troiani, Vanessa; Yerys, Benjamin E. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2016
In typical development there is a bias to orient visual attention to social information. Children with ASD do not reliably demonstrate this bias, and the role of attention orienting has not been well studied. We examined attention orienting via the inhibition of return (IOR) mechanism in a spatial cueing task using social-emotional cues; we…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Children, Child Development
Puspitawati, Ira; Jebrane, Ahmed; Vinter, Annie – Child Development, 2014
This study investigated the spatial analysis of tactile hierarchical patterns in 110 early-blind children aged 6-8 to 16-18 years, as compared to 90 blindfolded sighted children, in a naming and haptic drawing task. The results revealed that regardless of visual status, young children predominantly produced local responses in both tasks, whereas…
Descriptors: Blindness, Cognitive Processes, Child Development, Naming
Van der Molen, M. J.; Henry, L. A.; Van Luit, J. E. H. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2014
Background: The purpose of the current cross-sectional study was to examine the developmental progression in working memory (WM) between the ages of 9 and 16 years in a large sample of children with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities (MBID). Baddeley's influential WM model was used as a theoretical framework. Furthermore, the…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Mild Mental Retardation, Moderate Mental Retardation, Children
Huyck, Julia Jones; Wright, Beverly A. – Developmental Science, 2011
Adults can improve their performance on many perceptual tasks with training, but when does the response to training become mature? To investigate this question, we trained 11-year-olds, 14-year-olds and adults on a basic auditory task (temporal-interval discrimination) using a multiple-session training regimen known to be effective for adults. The…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Visual Perception, Cognitive Style, Age Differences
Williams, Joanne M. – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2012
This paper aims to provide developmental data on two connected naive inheritance concepts and to explore the coherence of children's naive biology knowledge. Two tasks examined children and adolescents' (4, 7, 10, and 14 years) conceptions of phenotypic resemblance across kin (in physical characteristics, disabilities, and personality traits). The…
Descriptors: Personality Traits, Age Differences, Biology, Physical Characteristics
Dumontheil, Iroise; Apperly, Ian A.; Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne – Developmental Science, 2010
The development of theory of mind use was investigated by giving a computerized task to 177 female participants divided into five age groups: Child I (7.3-9.7 years); Child II (9.8-11.4); Adolescent I (11.5-13.9); Adolescent II (14.0-17.7); Adults (19.1-27.5). Participants viewed a set of shelves containing objects, which they were instructed to…
Descriptors: Late Adolescents, Adolescents, Cognitive Development, Child Development
Ardila, Alfredo; Rosselli, Monica; Matute, Esmeralda; Inozemtseva, Olga – Developmental Psychology, 2011
The potential effect of gender on intellectual abilities remains controversial. The purpose of this research was to analyze gender differences in cognitive test performance among children from continuous age groups. For this purpose, the normative data from 7 domains of the newly developed neuropsychological test battery, the Evaluacion…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Cognitive Tests, Foreign Countries, Gender Differences
van Roon, Dominique; Caeyenberghs, Karen; Swinnen, Stephan P.; Smits-Engelsman, Bouwien C. M. – Child Development, 2008
To examine the development of feedforward control during manual tracking, 117 participants in 5 age groups (6 to 7, 8 to 9, 10 to 11, 12 to 14, and 15 to 17 years) tracked an accelerating dot presented on a monitor by moving an electronic pen on a digitizer. To remain successful at higher target velocities, they had to create a predictive model of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Adolescents, Visual Stimuli

Giray, Erol F.; Barclay, Allan G. – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1977
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Child Development, Children
Batty, Magali; Taylor, Margot J. – Developmental Science, 2006
Our facial expressions give others the opportunity to access our feelings, and constitute an important nonverbal tool for communication. Many recent studies have investigated emotional perception in adults, and our knowledge of neural processes involved in emotions is increasingly precise. Young children also use faces to express their internal…
Descriptors: Young Children, Child Development, Emotional Response, Nonverbal Communication
Robert, Michele; Heroux, Gisele – Infant and Child Development, 2004
This cross-sectional study explored whether participation, from early childhood, in play involving different cognitive abilities predicts visuo-spatial achievement at ages 9, 12, and 15. Based on parental assessment, prior and present practice of spatial manipulation play was found to be consistently more frequent in boys than in girls; the…
Descriptors: Play, Females, Spatial Ability, Males
Gerson, Arlene C.; Butler, Robert; Moxey-Mims, Marva; Wentz, Alicia; Shinnar, Shlomo; Lande, Marc B.; Mendley, Susan R.; Warady, Bradley A.; Furth, Susan L.; Hooper, Stephen R. – Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 2006
Given the rise in chronic kidney disease (CKD) in both children and adults, CKD has recently been targeted as a public health priority. Childhood onset kidney disease is generally a noncurable and progressive condition that leads to kidney failure by early adulthood. Fortunately, improved identification of kidney problems allows for early…
Descriptors: Early Intervention, Public Health, Chronic Illness, Quality of Life