Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 6 |
Correlation | 3 |
Language Skills | 3 |
Young Children | 3 |
Children | 2 |
Language Acquisition | 2 |
Language Impairments | 2 |
Predictor Variables | 2 |
Reading Difficulties | 2 |
Receptive Language | 2 |
Statistical Analysis | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Speech, Language,… | 2 |
Child Development | 1 |
First Language | 1 |
Journal of Early Intervention | 1 |
Journal of School Psychology | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Colorado (Denver) | 1 |
Europe | 1 |
Louisiana | 1 |
Russia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Boada, Katheryn L.; Boada, Richard; Pennington, Bruce F.; Peterson, Robin L. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2022
Purpose: Speech sound disorder (SSD) in conjunction with a language disorder has been associated with poor literacy acquisition; however, no study has evaluated whether articulation, phonological, or sequencing skills are differentially related to reading skills. Therefore, this study examined the relationship between speech error types at ages…
Descriptors: Speech Impairments, Language Impairments, Articulation Impairments, Phonological Awareness
Roy, Joseph; Oetting, Janna B.; Wynn Moland, Christy – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2013
Purpose: Overt marking of "BE" in nonmainstream adult dialects of English is influenced by a number of linguistic constraints, including the structure's person, number, tense, contractibility, and grammatical function. In the current study, the authors examined the effects of these constraints on overt marking of "BE" in…
Descriptors: Young Children, Black Dialects, African American Children, English
Gillis, Randall; Nilsen, Elizabeth S. – First Language, 2014
To become successful communicators, children must be sensitive to the clarity/ambiguity of language. Significant gains in children's ability to detect communicative ambiguity occur during the early school-age years. However, little is known about the cognitive abilities that support this development. Relations between cognitive flexibility and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Preschool Children, Language Acquisition, Ambiguity (Semantics)
Roben, Caroline K. P.; Cole, Pamela M.; Armstrong, Laura Marie – Child Development, 2013
Researchers have suggested that as children's language skill develops in early childhood, it comes to help children regulate their emotions (Cole, Armstrong, & Pemberton, 2010; Kopp, 1989), but the pathways by which this occurs have not been studied empirically. In a longitudinal study of 120 children from 18 to 48 months of age, associations…
Descriptors: Language Skills, Toddlers, Psychological Patterns, Self Control
Hough, Susan D.; Kaczmarek, Louise – Journal of Early Intervention, 2011
In recent years, many children from Eastern European orphanages have been adopted by families in the United States. When children begin life with their new families, they experience an abrupt language shift in which the learning of their native language halts as the learning of the new language commences. Without the support of their native…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Semantics, Syntax, Oral Language

Fodness, Ruth Wochnick; And Others – Journal of School Psychology, 1991
Examined test-retest reliability for Test of Language Development-2: Primary (TOLD-2 P) and Intermediate (TOLD-2 I). Findings from 60 children revealed that, with few exceptions, both tests had satisfactory reliability over 2-week interval. Less satisfactory reliability was found for TOLD-2 P Semantics Composite (ages 4, 6 ,and 8); Phonology…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Language Acquisition, Test Reliability, Young Children