Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 6 |
Classification | 6 |
Cognitive Development | 3 |
Children | 2 |
Young Children | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
Change | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
College Students | 1 |
Computation | 1 |
Developmental Stages | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 6 |
Author
Berger, Carole | 1 |
Bjorklund, David F. | 1 |
Coyle, Thomas R. | 1 |
Frazier, Brandy N. | 1 |
Gelman, A. | 1 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 1 |
Harris, Paul L. | 1 |
Hatwell, Yvette | 1 |
Kalish, Charles W. | 1 |
Kim, Sunae | 1 |
Mareschal, Denis | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Nguyen, Simone P.; Gelman, A. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Four studies examined the role of generic language in facilitating 4- and 5-year-old children's ability to cross-classify. Participants were asked to classify an item into a familiar (taxonomic or script) category, then cross-classify it into a novel (script or taxonomic) category with the help of a clue expressed in either generic or specific…
Descriptors: Classification, Generalization, Children, Experiments
Westermann, Gert; Mareschal, Denis – Cognitive Development, 2012
Computational models are tools for testing mechanistic theories of learning and development. Formal models allow us to instantiate theories of cognitive development in computer simulations. Model behavior can then be compared to real performance. Connectionist models, loosely based on neural information processing, have been successful in…
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Cognitive Development, Computation
Kim, Sunae; Kalish, Charles W.; Harris, Paul L. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Prior work shows that children can make inductive inferences about objects based on their labels rather than their appearance (Gelman, 2003). A separate line of research shows that children's trust in a speaker's label is selective. Children accept labels from a reliable speaker over an unreliable speaker (e.g., Koenig & Harris, 2005). In the…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Inferences, Classification, Young Children
Frazier, Brandy N.; Gelman, Susan A. – Cognitive Development, 2009
This study examined the development of an understanding of authenticity among 112 children (preschoolers, kindergarten, 1st graders, and 4th graders) and 119 college students. Participants were presented with pairs of photographs depicting authentic and non-authentic objects and asked to pick which one belongs in a museum and which one they would…
Descriptors: Research Methodology, Museums, Kindergarten, Grade 4
The Development of Strategic Memory: A Modified Microgenetic Assessment of Utilization Deficiencies.

Coyle, Thomas R.; Bjorklund, David F. – Cognitive Development, 1996
Classified children's use of cognitive strategies on a multitrial sort-recall task. Compared to fourth graders, more second and third graders were classified utilizationally deficient; fourth graders were more likely to be classified as quasi-utilizationally deficient. Levels of recall and clustering were higher for younger utilizationally…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages

Berger, Carole; Hatwell, Yvette – Cognitive Development, 1993
The developmental change from global toward dimensional classifications, usually observed in vision, was investigated in haptics with stimuli varying according to their size and roughness. Results indicated that, although more overall similarity classifications were observed in children than in adults, this kind of classification was never…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Classification