Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 50 |
Cognitive Development | 50 |
Infant Behavior | 50 |
Infants | 40 |
Comparative Analysis | 10 |
Classification | 8 |
Longitudinal Studies | 8 |
Visual Stimuli | 8 |
Child Development | 7 |
Perceptual Development | 7 |
Sex Differences | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Brooks, Rechele | 2 |
Fernald, Anne | 2 |
Mareschal, Denis | 2 |
McCall, Robert B. | 2 |
Meltzoff, Andrew N. | 2 |
Pinto, John P. | 2 |
Swingley, Daniel | 2 |
Werker, Janet F. | 2 |
Adams, Adria | 1 |
Adamson-Macedo, Elvidina | 1 |
Anderson, Diane | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 39 |
Journal Articles | 35 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 3 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meltzoff, Andrew N.; Murray, Lynne; Simpson, Elizabeth; Heimann, Mikael; Nagy, Emese; Nadel, Jacqueline; Pedersen, Eric J.; Brooks, Rechele; Messinger, Daniel S.; De Pascalis, Leonardo; Subiaul, Francys; Paukner, Annika; Ferrari, Pier F. – Developmental Science, 2018
The meaning, mechanism, and function of imitation in early infancy have been actively discussed since Meltzoff and Moore's (1977) report of facial and manual imitation by human neonates. Oostenbroek et al. (2016) claim to challenge the existence of early imitation and to counter all interpretations so far offered. Such claims, if true, would have…
Descriptors: Neonates, Human Body, Imitation, Infants
Mäkelä, Tiina E.; Peltola, Mikko J.; Nieminen, Pirkko; Paavonen, E. Juulia; Saarenpää-Heikkilä, Outi; Paunio, Tiina; Kylliäinen, Anneli – Developmental Psychology, 2018
Fragmented sleep is common in infancy. Although night awakening is known to decrease with age, in some infants night awakening is more persistent and continues into older ages. However, the influence of fragmented sleep on development is poorly known. In the present study, the longitudinal relationship between fragmented sleep and psychomotor…
Descriptors: Infants, Correlation, Psychomotor Skills, Sleep
Westermann, Gert; Mareschal, Denis – Cognitive Development, 2012
Computational models are tools for testing mechanistic theories of learning and development. Formal models allow us to instantiate theories of cognitive development in computer simulations. Model behavior can then be compared to real performance. Connectionist models, loosely based on neural information processing, have been successful in…
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Cognitive Development, Computation
Kingo, Osman S.; Krojgaard, Peter – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2012
This study investigates the importance of object function (action-object-outcome relations) on object individuation in infancy. Five experiments examined the ability of 9.5- and 12-month-old infants to individuate simple geometric objects in a manual search design. Experiments 1 through 4 (12-month-olds, N = 128) provided several combinations of…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Infants, Geometric Concepts, Experiments
Molfese, Victoria J.; Rudasill, Kathleen Moritz; Beswick, Jennifer L.; Jacobi-Vessels, Jill L.; Ferguson, Melissa C.; White, Jamie M. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2010
This study examined contributions of maternal personality and infant temperament to infant vocabulary and cognitive development both directly and indirectly through parental stress. Participants were recruited at birth and included 63 infant twin pairs and their mothers. Assessments were completed at 6, 9, 12, and 18 months of age and included…
Descriptors: Twins, Structural Equation Models, Child Rearing, Infants
Kaldy, Zsuzsa; Blaser, Erik – Infancy, 2009
What kind of featural information do infants rely on when they are trying to recognize a previously seen object? The question of whether infants use certain features (e.g., shape or color) more than others (e.g., luminance) can only be studied legitimately if visual salience is controlled, as the magnitude of feature values--how noticeable and…
Descriptors: Age, Identification, Infants, Visual Stimuli

Boller, Kimberly; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1990
Revealed that 6-month-old infants are unable to access either an original memory or a reactivated memory after lengthy intervals. Despite the fact that their memory processing is more rapid during encoding and retrieval than that of infants half their age, their facility for accessing an original or reactivated memory is weaker than that of such…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Context Effect, Infant Behavior

Horner, Thomas M. – Child Development, 1980
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Environmental Influences, Infant Behavior
Butterworth, George; Adamson-Macedo, Elvidina – 1987
Observations were made of the incidence of pointing behavior among infants differing in age when a radio-controlled truck entered the room in which mother and infant were seated. Infants' reactions over 5 to 6 minutes were recorded on videotape. Recordings were analyzed to establish the incidence of pointing, handedness of pointing, whether…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Foreign Countries, Incidence

Tilford, Jane A. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1976
Compares infants' performance based on chronological age as opposed to conceptual age. The Bayley Mental Scales were used to test 20 premature experimental infants and 20 full-term control infants. Results demonstrated that an infant's performance must be judged on the basis of conceptual rather than chronological age. (MS)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Early Childhood Education, Infant Behavior

Booth, Amy E.; Waxman, Sandra – Developmental Psychology, 2002
Two studies examined whether object names and functions act as cues to categories for infants. Findings indicated that both 14- and 18-month-olds were more likely to select the category match after being shown a novel category exemplar with its function than when given no additional cues. Only at 18 months did naming the objects enhance…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis

Santelmann, Lynn M.; Jusczyk, Peter W. – Cognition, 1998
Five experiments examined 15- and 18-month olds' sensitivity to morphosyntactic dependencies. Results indicated that 18-month olds, but not 15-month olds, were sensitive to basic relationship between "is" and "-ing" and that 18-month-olds could track relationships between functor morphemes. Findings were consistent with hypothesis that 18-month…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, English, Infant Behavior

Kavanaugh, Robert D.; Eizenman, Dara R.; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Psychology, 1997
Studied 2-year olds' understanding of pretense expressions of independent agency in scenarios in which a doll acted as the agent of a series of pretend events. Found no gender differences in the doll's imaginary intentions, but older toddlers performed reliably better than younger. Episodes requiring enacting conclusions to events that began with…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Intention

Patterson, Michelle L.; Werker, Janet F. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2002
Tested in six experiments young infants' sensitivity to vowel and gender information in faces and voices. Found that 4.5-month-olds showed no evidence of matching face and voice based on gender, but were able to ignore irrelevant gender information and match based on the vowel. Robust evidence of ability to match based on gender was not evident…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Infants
Xu, Fei; Baker, Allison – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2005
Several investigators find that infants fail to use property information to individuate objects until 12 months of age (e.g., Xu & Carey, 1996), while others find that infants successfully employ property information in the service of object individuation at 9.5 months (e.g., Wilcox & Baillargeon, 1998a). This study investigated…
Descriptors: Infants, Experiments, Child Development, Age Differences