NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Researchers1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Kit of Reference Tests for…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 18 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zhang, Songshan; Zhang, Xian – Language Teaching Research, 2022
This study set out to investigate the relationship between L2 vocabulary knowledge (VK) and second-language (L2) reading/listening comprehension. More than 100 individual studies were included in this meta-analysis, which generated 276 effect sizes from a sample of almost 21,000 learners. The current meta-analysis had several major findings.…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Vocabulary Development, Listening Comprehension, Reading Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hupp, Julie M. – Infant and Child Development, 2015
Attention allocation in word learning may vary developmentally based on the novelty of the object. It has been suggested that children differentially learn verbs based on the novelty of the agent, but adults do not because they automatically infer the object's category and thus treat it like a familiar object. The current research examined…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Adults, Children, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Allen, Michael – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2015
Although taxonomic proficiency is a prerequisite for understanding ideas central to biology, previous research has established that learners frequently misclassify animals by not following the tenets of accepted taxonomic rubrics. This has immediate relevance with the recently revised English National Curriculum now requiring concepts of animal…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Knowledge Level, Animals, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wagner, Laura; Vega-Mendoza, Mariana; Van Horn, Suzanne – First Language, 2014
Speakers must command different linguistic registers to index various social-discourse elements, including the identity of the addressee. Previous work found that English-learning children could link registers to appropriate addressees by 5 years. Two experiments found that better cues to the linguistic form or to the social meaning of register…
Descriptors: Cues, Social Influences, English, Spanish
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Byrne, Jenny – International Journal of Science Education, 2011
This paper describes the expressed models that children aged 7, 11, and 14 years have about micro-organisms and microbial activity. These were elicited using a variety of data collection techniques that complemented each other, resulting in a rich dataset, and provided information about the level of knowledge and progression of ideas across the…
Descriptors: Knowledge Level, Microbiology, Scientific Concepts, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Holub, Shayla C.; Musher-Eizenman, Dara R. – Early Child Development and Care, 2010
Eating behaviours begin to develop during early childhood, but relatively little is known about preschoolers' nutrition knowledge. The current study examined age and gender differences in this knowledge using two tasks: food group classification and the creation of unhealthy, healthy and preferred meals. Sixty-nine three- to six-year-old children…
Descriptors: Interviews, Knowledge Level, Nutrition, Eating Habits
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Woolard, Jennifer L.; Cleary, Hayley M. D.; Harvell, Samantha A. S.; Chen, Rusan – Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 2008
This study examines whether parents have the prerequisite knowledge about police interrogation that would allow them to compensate for youths' knowledge deficits, protect their interests, and buffer against their vulnerability to coercion. A racially diverse urban/suburban convenience sample of 77 11- to 13-year-olds, 46 14- to 15-year-olds, and…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Economically Disadvantaged, Intelligence Quotient, Parent Child Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Liu, Jing; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Sak, Kimberly – Child Development, 2001
Six match-to-sample picture/object selection experiments explored 3- to 5-year-olds' knowledge about superordinate words and acquisition of this knowledge. Findings indicated that number of standards (one versus two), types of standards (different versus same basic-level categories), and nature of representation (pictures versus objects)…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Kathy E.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1992
Judgments of similarity among mammals, including humans, were elicited from 7 and 10 year olds and adults. Results partially supported a consensus model of shared cultural knowledge. Patterns of deviation from the model appeared between children and adults because of the emergence of a primate category after age 10. (BC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moynahan, Eileen D. – Child Development, 1973
Results indicates that awareness of the facilitative effect of categorization on recall increases with age, particularly during the early grade-school years. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Elementary School Students, Knowledge Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Farrar, Michael Jeffrey; And Others – Child Development, 1992
In one experiment, second and fourth graders used more categorical information when they made inferences than did preschoolers. In two other experiments, second graders, but not preschoolers, distinguished between categorical information and appearance when they made inferences about known concepts and familiar properties. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lane, Mary Kay; Hodkin, Barbara – Developmental Psychology, 1985
Demonstrates the usefulness of the inclusion paradigm as a methodological tool in providing information about the conceptual breadth of selected social and nonsocial superordinate categories in children who exhibit some degree of inclusion logic. (HOD)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Classification, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huxham, Mark; Welsh, Angela; Berry, Alice; Templeton, Stuart – Journal of Biological Education, 2006
We examined the wildlife knowledge of primary (aged 4-12) schoolchildren. In particular, we examined the effects of children's age and gender, as well as the taxonomy and origin (indigenous versus exotic) of wildlife, on the degree of knowledge about different species. We used illustrated "flashcards" of mammals, birds and arthropods,…
Descriptors: Animals, Wildlife, Performance Factors, Knowledge Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Kathy E.; Scott, Paul; Mervis, Carolyn B. – Developmental Psychology, 1997
Four studies examined developmental differences in the representation of basic-subordinate inclusion relationships in three-, five-, and seven-year olds and undergraduates. Found that even three-year olds showed rudimentary knowledge of the asymmetry of inclusion. There was a marked developmental gap between producing subordinate category names…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Development, Children
O'Hara, Takeshi; And Others – 1978
Path analysis was used to reanalyze Kropp and Stoker's data from tests designed to evaluate Bloom's taxonomy of educational objectives in the cognitive domain. Scores for 1,128 students in grades nine through twelve were analyzed separately by grade level for four content areas on six taxonomic levels. A measure of general ability was also…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Objectives
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2