Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 4 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 8 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 13 |
Cognitive Development | 13 |
Language Usage | 13 |
Preschool Children | 6 |
Comparative Analysis | 5 |
Children | 4 |
Language Acquisition | 4 |
Young Children | 3 |
Academic Achievement | 2 |
Adults | 2 |
COVID-19 | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Cate Smith Todd | 2 |
Emily Swinth | 2 |
Lydia Carlis | 2 |
Maya Goldberg | 2 |
Meghan McCormick | 2 |
Victoria Chavez | 2 |
Alfieri, P. | 1 |
Austin, Alison C. | 1 |
Blake, Joanna | 1 |
Cengiz, Özge | 1 |
Cheung, Kar Man | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 13 |
Journal Articles | 11 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Head Start | 2 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Woolley, Jacqueline D.; Kelley, Kelsey A. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
In Study 1, 103 children ages 4 through 10 answered questions about their concept of and belief in luck, and completed a story task assessing their use of luck as an explanation for events. The interview captured a curvilinear trajectory of children's belief in luck from tentative belief at age 4 to full belief at age 6, weakening belief at age 8,…
Descriptors: Children, Concept Formation, Beliefs, Child Development
Vasil, Jared; Moore, Charlotte; Tomasello, Michael – First Language, 2023
Shared intentionality theory posits that at age 3, children expand their conception of plural agency to include 3- or more-person groups. We sought to determine whether this conceptual shift is detectable in children's pronoun use. We report the results of a series of Bayesian hierarchical generative models fitted to 479 English-speaking…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Preschool Children, Language Acquisition, Language Usage
Meghan McCormick; Maya Goldberg; Emily Swinth; Cate Smith Todd; Lydia Carlis; Victoria Chavez – MDRC, 2023
There is clear evidence that the COVID-19 pandemic had significant negative effects on the learning and development of school-age children in the United States, with disproportionate impacts on children from racially, ethnically, and socioeconomically marginalized groups. There is less consistent evidence on the extent to which the pandemic…
Descriptors: COVID-19, Pandemics, Preschool Children, Preschool Education
Meghan McCormick; Maya Goldberg; Emily Swinth; Cate Smith Todd; Lydia Carlis; Victoria Chavez – Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2023
Background/rationale: There is clear evidence that the COVID-19 pandemic had significant negative effects on the learning and development of school-age children in the United States, with disproportionate impacts on children from racially, ethnically, and socioeconomically marginalized groups (Irwin et al., 2022). There is less consistent evidence…
Descriptors: COVID-19, Pandemics, Preschool Children, Preschool Education
Austin, Alison C.; Schuler, Kathryn D.; Furlong, Sarah; Newport, Elissa L. – Language Learning and Development, 2022
When linguistic input contains inconsistent use of grammatical forms, children produce these forms more consistently, a process called "regularization." Deaf children learning American Sign Language from parents who are non-native users of the language regularize their parents' inconsistent usages. In studies of artificial languages…
Descriptors: Linguistic Input, Deafness, Age Differences, Language Acquisition
Leach, Jamie; Howe, Nina; DeHart, Ganie – Infant and Child Development, 2017
The present study investigated children's internal state language during play with their sibling and friend across early and middle childhood. Specifically, the category type of internal state language (e.g., cognitions and goals), referent (e.g., own and other), and associations with children's birth order were examined. A total of 65 (T1: Time…
Descriptors: Play, Sibling Relationship, Peer Relationship, Birth Order
Alfieri, P.; Menghini, D.; Marotta, L.; De Peppo, L.; Ravà, L.; Salvaguardia, F.; Varuzza, C.; Vicari, S. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2017
Background: Individuals with Williams syndrome (WS) show a disharmonic linguistic profile with a clear pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Despite their sociable nature, atypical socio-communicative abilities and deficits in communication and relationship with others have been found. Aim: The aim of the present study was to investigate whether…
Descriptors: Intellectual Disability, Genetic Disorders, Language Skills, Interpersonal Communication
Çakir, Hamide; Cengiz, Özge – Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 2016
Parent-child interactions and characteristics of mothers' child-directed language have been related to children's linguistic development. Studies on parent-child interactions in Turkey have generally focused on children. There have not been many researches on Turkish motherese. This study addresses this gap by exploring the properties of Turkish…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Parent Child Relationship, Mothers, Interaction

Dimcovic, N.; Tobin, M. J. – Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 1995
Verbal and figurative classification tasks were presented to 30 blind and 30 sighted children (ages 6 to 11). Although younger blind children were significantly less efficient on tasks, older ones reached or were close to the level of their sighted peers. Analysis illustrates how the blind children adjusted their conceptual knowledge to their…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Blindness, Children, Classification

Blake, Joanna; And Others – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1994
Preschool children were given a memory task that required repeating a list of animal names and a sentence imitation task. Results confirmed a relationship between word span and language imitation in younger preschool children and the notion of a memory constraint on early spontaneous language. Increasing mastery of linguistic rules appeared to…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Language Aptitude

Waller, Glenn – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Two experiments showed that: 5- and 6-year-old listeners have difficulties with spatial reference if it includes "left" and "right"; and 7-year-olds understand this limitation on the comprehension skill of younger children and make appropriate allowances by using more landmarks instead. (CB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Language Processing, Language Usage
Tardif, Twila; Wellman, Henry M.; Cheung, Kar Man – Journal of Child Language, 2004
The present study investigates the performance of 96 Cantonese-speaking three- to five-year-old preschoolers on three false belief tasks--a deceptive object, a change of location, and an unexpected contents task encompassing a variety of task factors. Most importantly, the research examines the possibility that false belief performance depends on…
Descriptors: Sino Tibetan Languages, Child Language, Preschool Children, Verbs

Yumoto, Kazuko – Kanagawa University Language Studies, 1984
A naturalistic study looked at the acquisition of English by two Japanese boys, aged 4 and 8 years, during a 2.5-year stay in the United States. Data were collected through observation and transcription of spontaneous speech in daily life. Analysis included a variety of features of language use and of the acquisition process, including attitudes…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Bilingualism, Child Language, Code Switching (Language)