Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 13 |
Developmental Psychology | 13 |
Predictor Variables | 13 |
Correlation | 5 |
Adolescents | 4 |
Longitudinal Studies | 4 |
Questionnaires | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Multiple Regression Analysis | 3 |
Parent Child Relationship | 3 |
Preschool Children | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
British Journal of… | 4 |
Developmental Psychology | 3 |
Family Relations | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Applied… | 1 |
Journal of Cognition and… | 1 |
Perceptual and Motor Skills | 1 |
Research Quarterly for… | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 13 |
Reports - Research | 12 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Secondary Education | 3 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 11 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Grade 9 | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Hopkins Symptom Checklist | 2 |
Classroom Behavior Inventory | 1 |
Early Childhood Environment… | 1 |
Rothbart Infant Behavior… | 1 |
Student Teacher Relationship… | 1 |
UCLA Loneliness Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Smith, Eric D.; Lillard, Angeline S. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2012
Piaget (1962) asserted that children stop engaging in pretend play when they enter the concrete operational stage because they become able to accommodate reality and no longer need to assimilate it to their wishes. Consistent also with the views of Vygotsky, discussion of pretend play in developmental psychology is typically confined to early…
Descriptors: Children, Play, Developmental Psychology, Investigations
Van Petegem, Stijn; Vansteenkiste, Maarten; Soenens, Bart; Beyers, Wim; Aelterman, Nathalie – Developmental Psychology, 2015
In this longitudinal study, we tested whether the association between oppositional defiance to parental authority (i.e., adolescents' tendency to bluntly reject parental rules) and autonomy would depend upon the specific conceptualization of autonomy. Whereas oppositional defiance would yield more interpersonal distance from parents, because…
Descriptors: Longitudinal Studies, Developmental Psychology, Correlation, Behavior Disorders
Naerde, Ane; Ogden, Terje; Janson, Harald; Zachrisson, Henrik Daae – Developmental Psychology, 2014
This study investigated the normative use and developmental course of physical aggression (PA), defined as use of physical force such as hitting, biting, and kicking, from 8 to 26 months and predictors thereof. We used data from the Behavior Outlook Norwegian Developmental Study, comprising 1,159 children (559 girls and 600 boys). Both mothers and…
Descriptors: Aggression, Developmental Stages, Developmental Psychology, Toddlers
Schietecatte, Inge; Roeyers, Herbert; Warreyn, Petra – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2012
From the moment infants are born, they seem to prefer orienting to social stimuli, over objects and non-social stimuli. This preference lasts throughout adulthood and is believed to play a crucial role in social-communicative development. By following up a group of infants at the age of 6, 8, and 12 months, this study explored the role of social…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Infants, Orientation, Attention
Le, Vi-Nhuan; Schaack, Diana; Setodji, Claude Messan – Developmental Psychology, 2015
Many child care centers temporarily move children and teachers in and out of their assigned classrooms throughout the day. Such practices create frequent discontinuity in children's experiences in child care, including discontinuity in their peer and teacher relationships. This study examined the prevalence and patterns of teacher and child…
Descriptors: Developmental Psychology, Correlation, Child Caregivers, Child Care Centers
Abebe, Dawit Shawel; Torgersen, Leila; Lien, Lars; Hafstad, Gertrud S.; von Soest, Tilmann – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2014
We investigated longitudinal predictors for disordered eating from early adolescence to young adulthood (12-34 years) across gender and different developmental phases among Norwegian young people. Survey data from a population-based sample were collected at four time points (T) over a 13-year time span. A population-based sample of 5,679 females…
Descriptors: Longitudinal Studies, Predictor Variables, Eating Disorders, Adolescents
Hill, Patrick L.; Burrow, Anthony L.; Brandenberger, Jay W.; Lapsley, Daniel K.; Quaranto, Jessica Collado – Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2010
Two studies evaluated whether different purpose orientations, defined by the content of one's life-goals, would differentially predict personal well-being in the short- and long-term. Four types of purpose orientations (creative, prosocial, financial, and personal recognition) were examined using a sample of 416 (57% male) college undergraduates…
Descriptors: Integrity, Age Differences, Adults, Young Adults
Weiss, Windee M.; Weiss, Maureen R. – Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 2007
The purpose of this study was to examine age and competitive level differences in the relationship between determinants and level of sport commitment. Gymnasts (N = 304) comprised three age groups (8-11, 11-14.5, and 14.5-18 years) and two competitive levels (Levels 5-6 and 8-10). Multiple regression analyses revealed: (a) perceived costs and…
Descriptors: Motivation, Competition, Females, Athletics

Goebel, Barbara L.; Harris, Elizabeth L. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1980
Results of this study of three age groups (5-7, 9-10, and 14-15 years) indicate that there are both individual and developmental differences in cognitive strategy use and that use of strategy is predictable across age levels by personality factors. (Author/SJL)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Style
McAlister, Anna; Peterson, Candida C. – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2006
This study assessed the theory of mind (ToM) and executive functioning (EF) abilities of 124 typically developing preschool children aged 3 to 5 years in relation to whether or not they had a child-aged sibling (i.e. a child aged 1 to 12 years) at home with whom to play and converse. On a ToM battery that included tests of false belief,…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Siblings, Preschool Children, Multiple Regression Analysis

Reis, Janet; And Others – Family Relations, 1986
Used an ecological model of human development to assess the interrelationship between psychological determinants of parenting, parental age, and parenting skill for a sample of parents (N=310). Found parents' race to be significantly related to attitudes toward childrearing, knowledge of child development, and perceived social support. (Author/ABB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Rearing, Developmental Psychology, Ecological Factors
Raikes, H. Abigail; Thompson, Ross A. – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2006
Despite its prevalence in low-income populations, there has been little attention paid to how maternal depression influences mother-child conversations about emotions and low-income preschool children's developing emotion understanding. The importance of a secure attachment as a positive influence on emotion understanding has also been…
Descriptors: Mothers, Preschool Children, Attachment Behavior, Depression (Psychology)
Krettenauer, Tobias; Eichler, Dana – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2006
The study investigates adolescents' self-attributed moral emotions following a moral transgression by expanding research with children on the happy-victimizer phenomenon. In a sample of 200 German adolescents from Grades 7, 9, 11, and 13 (M=16.18 years, SD=2.41), participants were confronted with various scenarios describing different moral rule…
Descriptors: Social Desirability, Adolescents, Value Judgment, Moral Development