NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Location
Canada1
China1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing all 14 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lin, Jing; Zhang, Letong; Neumann, Knut; Cheng, Ping-Han; Wei, Wenting; Chang, Chun-Yen – Asia-Pacific Science Education, 2022
Scientific modeling (SM) is a core practice of science and an important component of scientific literacy. Supporting students in developing the competence to construct, use, evaluate, and revise models is hence of particular relevance. While research has shown that spatial visualization (SV), a core component of spatial ability, is correlated with…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Visualization, Scientific Concepts, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Putland, Jennifer; Hoeberechts, Maia; Pelz, Monika; Hudson, Lauren; Tolmie, Cody; Carrasquilla-Henao, Mauricio – Canadian Journal of Environmental Education, 2021
Formal climate education without consideration of the ocean is incomplete. The effectiveness of a new climate lesson for youth that includes the ocean-climate nexus was examined by delivering the lesson to nine classes situated in separate British Columbia, Canada public schools and assessing the students' understanding of basic climate concepts…
Descriptors: Oceanography, Climate, Environmental Education, Sustainable Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Allen, Michael – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2015
Although taxonomic proficiency is a prerequisite for understanding ideas central to biology, previous research has established that learners frequently misclassify animals by not following the tenets of accepted taxonomic rubrics. This has immediate relevance with the recently revised English National Curriculum now requiring concepts of animal…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Knowledge Level, Animals, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hast, Michael; Howe, Christine – Journal of Science Education and Technology, 2013
Previous research indicates children reason in different ways about horizontal motion and motion in fall. At the same time, their understanding of motion down inclines appears to result from an interaction between horizontal and vertical motion understanding. However, this interaction is still poorly understood. Understanding of speed change may…
Descriptors: Scientific Concepts, Science Education, Elementary School Science, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Andrews, Glenda; Halford, Graeme S.; Murphy, Karen; Knox, Kathy – Cognitive Development, 2009
Young children's integration of weight and distance information was examined using a new methodology that combines a single-armed apparatus with functional measurement. Weight and distance values were varied factorially across the item set. Children estimated how far the beam would tilt when different numbers of weights were placed at different…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Measurement, Thinking Skills, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ebersbach, Mirjam; Resing, Wilma C. M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
Two experiments using the "projection of shadows" paradigm investigated multidimensional reasoning, implicit and explicit knowledge, and the nonlinearity concept in 5-, 9-, and 13-year-olds and adults. Participants estimated the resulting shadow lengths of differently sized objects, placed at varying distances from a light source. Experiment 1…
Descriptors: Knowledge Level, Children, Early Adolescents, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Au, Terry Kit-fong; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1993
The results of four studies involving three to seven year olds revealed that, by age three, some children (1) appreciated conservation of matter despite visual disappearance and the existence of invisible particles; and (2) made use of the particle concept to explain how a particle can continue to exist and maintain its properties despite visual…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Conservation (Concept), Scientific Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Friedman, William J. – Child Development, 2001
Three experiments examined 3- to 11-year-olds' understanding of entropy, asking whether undifferentiated forces, such as the wind or objects being thrown into the air, could create order or disorder in a set of objects. Found that even 4-year-olds were sensitive to asymmetrical effects of such events. Older children applied this principle more…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Friedman, William J. – Child Development, 2000
Four studies explored children's ability to differentiate future distances of events. Findings indicated that 4-year-olds failed to differentiate future distances. Five-year-olds could distinguish events occurring in coming weeks/months from those many months away. Six- through 8-year-olds made more differentiated judgments than younger children…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Intervals
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Levin, Iris – Child Development, 1979
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cues, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Winer, Gerald A.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1996
Three studies used computer graphics and/or verbal questioning to examine beliefs among children and adults that vision involves input to the eyes (intromission) or emissions from the eye (extramission). Results showed decreases in extramission and increases in intromission beliefs across age. There were more extramission interpretations with…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Beliefs, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hood, Bruce M. – Cognitive Development, 1995
Tested children with apparatus that dropped balls through clear or opaque interwoven tubes. Found that older children could solve configurations with greater number of tubes than younger children. Success with clear tubes did not transfer to opaque tubes. Significantly, errors were consistently directed to location directly below ball's last seen…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Structures
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gelman, Susan A. – Young Children, 1998
Reviews selected research on children's early formation of categories. Finds sophistication in how children group objects and think about those groupings. Notes findings related to type of grouping (thematic or taxonomic), multiple classifications, overgeneralization, the role of background knowledge on classification abilities, the…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Classification, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Solomon, Joan – School Science Review, 1983
Discusses students' formation of concepts about energy, classifying responses as living/nonliving, human (vitalism or activity), and nonhuman (supply or demand). Observations were based on class discussions and free writing, followed by questioning to check on stability of concepts. Also collected and reported are student responses (examples of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation