Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 5 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 7 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 12 |
Descriptor
Arithmetic | 17 |
Elementary School Students | 8 |
Foreign Countries | 8 |
Mathematics Skills | 8 |
Problem Solving | 8 |
Adults | 5 |
Mathematics Education | 5 |
Mathematics Instruction | 5 |
Numeracy | 5 |
Computation | 4 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
LeFevre, Jo-Anne | 17 |
Xu, Chang | 8 |
Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina | 5 |
Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn | 5 |
Douglas, Heather | 4 |
Maloney, Erin A. | 4 |
Osana, Helena P. | 4 |
Simms, Victoria | 4 |
Wylie, Judith | 4 |
Lafay, Anne | 3 |
Bisanz, Jeffrey | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 15 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 7 |
Early Childhood Education | 5 |
Primary Education | 5 |
Grade 2 | 4 |
Grade 3 | 4 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Keymath Diagnostic Arithmetic… | 1 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Child Development, 2021
How do children develop associations among number symbols? For Grade 1 children (n = 66, M = 78 months), sequence knowledge (i.e., identify missing numbers) and number comparison (i.e., choose larger number) predicted addition, both concurrently and indirectly at the end of Grade 1. Number ordering (i.e., touch numbers in order) did not predict…
Descriptors: Children, Numeracy, Symbols (Mathematics), Elementary School Students
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; Maloney, Erin A.; Wylie, Judith; Simms, Victoria; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Osana, Helena P. – Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2023
Children's knowledge of the ordinal relations among number symbols is related to their mathematical learning. Ordinal knowledge has been measured using judgment (i.e., decide whether a sequence of three digits is in order) and ordering tasks (i.e., order three digits from smallest to largest). However, the question remains whether performance on…
Descriptors: Young Children, Numeracy, Number Concepts, Serial Ordering
Xu, Chang; Lafay, Anne; Douglas, Heather; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Osana, Helena P.; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Wylie, Judith; Simms, Victoria; Maloney, Erin A. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2022
Language skills play an important role in mathematics development. Students (7 to 10 years of age) learning school mathematics either in the same language used at home (first-language learners; n = 103) or in a different language (second-language learners; n = 57) participated in the study. Relations among cognitive skills (i.e., receptive…
Descriptors: Mathematics Skills, Language Skills, Arithmetic, Word Problems (Mathematics)
Susperreguy, María Inés; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; Xu, Chang; Douglas, Heather; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Child Development, 2020
This study investigated the longitudinal associations between children's early mathematics and their home numeracy environment (HNE). Chilean children from families who varied widely in socioeconomic status were assessed at the beginning and end of prekindergarten in 2016 (N = 419, M[subscript age] = 4:7 [years:months]), and at the end of…
Descriptors: Kindergarten, Young Children, Family Environment, Numeracy
Xu, Chang; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Douglas, Heather; Lafay, Anne; Osana, Helena P.; Simms, Victoria; Wylie, Judith; Maloney, Erin A.; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2022
Canadian students enrolled in either French-immersion or English-instruction programs were followed from Grades 2 to 3 (M[subscript age] = 7.8 years to 8.9 years; N = 244; 55% girls). In each grade, students completed two mathematical tasks that required oral language processing (i.e., word-problem solving and number transcoding from dictation)…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Immersion Programs, French, English
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Di Lonardo Burr, Sabrina; Lafay, Anne; Wylie, Judith; Osana, Helena P.; Douglas, Heather; Maloney, Erin A.; Simms, Victoria – Developmental Psychology, 2021
In the present research, we provide empirical evidence for the process of symbolic integration of number associations, focusing on the development of simple addition (e.g., 5 + 3 = 8), subtraction (e.g., 5 - 3 = 2), and multiplication (e.g., 5 × 3 = 15). Canadian children were assessed twice, in Grade 2 and Grade 3 (N = 244; 55% girls). All…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Arithmetic, Mathematics Skills, Age Differences
Xu, Chang; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Are there differential benefits of training sequential number knowledge versus spatial skills for children's numerical and spatial performance? Three- to five-year-old children (N = 84) participated in 1 session of either sequential training (e.g., what comes before and after the number 5?) or non-numerical spatial training (i.e., decomposition of…
Descriptors: Young Children, Preschool Children, Numbers, Mathematics
Robinson, Katherine M.; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2012
Researchers have speculated that children find it more difficult to acquire conceptual understanding of the inverse relation between multiplication and division than that between addition and subtraction. We reviewed research on children and adults' use of shortcut procedures that make use of the inverse relation on two kinds of problems:…
Descriptors: Problem Solving, Mathematical Concepts, Multiplication, Arithmetic
Xu, Chang; Wells, Emma; LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Imbo, Ineke – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The purpose of the present study was to examine factors that influence strategic flexibility in computational estimation for Chinese- and Canadian-educated adults. Strategic flexibility was operationalized as the percentage of trials on which participants chose the problem-based procedure that best balanced proximity to the correct answer with…
Descriptors: Computation, Problem Solving, Feedback (Response), Accuracy
Pyke, Aryn A.; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2011
Why is subsequent recall sometimes better for self-generated answers than for answers obtained from an external source (e.g., calculator)? In this study, we explore the relative contribution of 2 processes, recall attempts and self-computation, to this "generation effect" (i.e., enhanced answer recall relative to when problems are practiced with a…
Descriptors: Mental Computation, Calculators, Arithmetic, Recall (Psychology)
LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Berrigan, Lindsay; Vendetti, Corrie; Kamawar, Deepthi; Bisanz, Jeffrey; Skwarchuk, Sheri-Lynn; Smith-Chant, Brenda L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
We examined the role of executive attention, which encompasses the common aspects of executive function and executive working memory, in children's acquisition of two aspects of mathematical skill: (a) knowledge of the number system (e.g., place value) and of arithmetic procedures (e.g., multi-digit addition) and (b) arithmetic fluency (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Number Concepts, Number Systems, Executive Function
Imbo, Ineke; LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
In the present study, the authors tested the effects of working-memory load on math problem solving in 3 different cultures: Flemish-speaking Belgians, English-speaking Canadians, and Chinese-speaking Chinese currently living in Canada. Participants solved complex addition problems (e.g., 58 + 76) in no-load and working-memory load conditions, in…
Descriptors: Cultural Differences, Arithmetic, Problem Solving, Short Term Memory

LeFevre, Jo-Anne – Mathematical Cognition, 1998
Presents a study in which 32 Chinese-speaking adults solved single-digit multiplication problems. Chinese adults made more errors that indicated interactions between phonological codes activated at encoding and production of answers as compared to samples of English, French, and Dutch-speaking adults from other studies. Contains 26 references.…
Descriptors: Adult Basic Education, Adults, Arithmetic, Cross Cultural Studies

LeFevre, Jo-Anne; Liu, Jing – Mathematical Cognition, 1997
Examines adults from China and Canada solving single-digit multiplication problems. Reports that Chinese adults were faster and made fewer errors than Canadian adults, and Chinese adults made more errors that reflect verbal-production processes that may occur after retrieval whereas Canadian adults made more errors that reflect retrieval…
Descriptors: Adult Learning, Arithmetic, Computation, Cross Cultural Studies

LeFevre, Jo-Anne; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1991
Children in grades three through five and adults performed a number-matching task. Found that changes in the strength of arithmetic connections occurred with development and accounted for individual differences among adults. Individual differences among children were related to changes in the strength of number-line connections. (Author/GLR)
Descriptors: Arithmetic, College Students, Developmental Stages, Elementary Education
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2