Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 15 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 35 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 92 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 297 |
Descriptor
Association (Psychology) | 832 |
Cognitive Processes | 198 |
Memory | 166 |
Recall (Psychology) | 166 |
Associative Learning | 104 |
Semantics | 94 |
Learning Processes | 86 |
Foreign Countries | 79 |
Models | 63 |
Cues | 60 |
College Students | 59 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 78 |
Postsecondary Education | 47 |
Early Childhood Education | 12 |
Elementary Education | 11 |
Preschool Education | 10 |
Secondary Education | 10 |
High Schools | 8 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Grade 10 | 3 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Grade 5 | 3 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Practitioners | 16 |
Researchers | 12 |
Teachers | 4 |
Location
Canada | 9 |
Germany | 9 |
Israel | 9 |
Australia | 8 |
United States | 6 |
China | 5 |
Pennsylvania | 4 |
Turkey | 4 |
United Kingdom (England) | 4 |
California | 3 |
Georgia | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Bilingual Education Act 1968 | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Bugg, Julie M.; Hutchison, Keith A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Prior studies have shown that cognitive control is implemented at the list and context levels in the color-word Stroop task. At first blush, the finding that Stroop interference is reduced for mostly incongruent items as compared with mostly congruent items (i.e., the item-specific proportion congruence [ISPC] effect) appears to provide evidence…
Descriptors: Color, Naming, Word Recognition, Association (Psychology)
Morais, Ana Sofia; Olsson, Henrik; Schooler, Lael J. – Cognitive Science, 2013
Aggregating snippets from the semantic memories of many individuals may not yield a good map of an individual's semantic memory. The authors analyze the structure of semantic networks that they sampled from individuals through a new snowball sampling paradigm during approximately 6 weeks of 1-hr daily sessions. The semantic networks of individuals…
Descriptors: Memory, Semantics, Interviews, Association (Psychology)
Fischer, Martin H.; Shaki, Samuel – Brain and Cognition, 2011
The commentary by Treccani and Umilta (2011) on our recent paper in this journal (Fischer et al., 2010) usefully broadens the perspective on numerical cognition for a general readership and clarifies (or raises) some fundamental issues of cognitive representation. Our response to this commentary is organized into three main sections that focus…
Descriptors: Numbers, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Association (Psychology)
McFarlane, Kimberley A.; Humphreys, Michael S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Research with the maintenance-rehearsal paradigm, in which word pairs are rehearsed as distractor material during a series of digit recall trials, has previously indicated that low frequency and new word pairs capture attention to a greater degree than high frequency and old word pairs. This impacts delayed recognition of the pairs and interferes…
Descriptors: Memory, Research, Attention, Role
Ginsburg, Véronique; van Dijck, Jean-Philippe; Previtali, Paola; Fias, Wim; Gevers, Wim – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Spatial-numerical associations are observed when participants perform number categorization tasks. One such observation is the spatial numerical associations of response codes (SNARC) effect, showing an association between small numbers and the left-hand side and between large numbers and the right-hand side. It has long been argued that this…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Short Term Memory, Verbal Ability, Numbers
Lerner, Itamar; Bentin, Shlomo; Shriki, Oren – Cognitive Science, 2012
Localist models of spreading activation (SA) and models assuming distributed representations offer very different takes on semantic priming, a widely investigated paradigm in word recognition and semantic memory research. In this study, we implemented SA in an attractor neural network model with distributed representations and created a unified…
Descriptors: Priming, Memory, Models, Word Recognition
Lakens, Daniel; Semin, Gun R.; Foroni, Francesco – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
Light and dark are used pervasively to represent positive and negative concepts. Recent studies suggest that black and white stimuli are automatically associated with negativity and positivity. However, structural factors in experimental designs, such as the shared opposition in the valence (good vs. bad) and brightness (light vs. dark) dimensions…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Color, Psychological Patterns, Cognitive Structures
Marshall, Nigel; Shibazaki, Kagari – Music Education Research, 2013
This paper reports on the results of a study carried out with 105 children, aged between three and four years in three nursery units in London and Surrey, UK. The aim of this study was to explore the level of association which young children have between various musical instruments, musical styles and a particular gender. However, we also aimed to…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Preschool Children, Musical Instruments, Music
MacKenzie, Heather; Curtin, Suzanne; Graham, Susan A. – Developmental Science, 2012
A fundamental step in learning words is the development of an association between a sound pattern and an element in the environment. Here we explore the nature of this associative ability in 12-month-olds, examining whether it is constrained to privilege particular word forms over others. Forty-eight infants were presented with sets of novel…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, Association (Psychology), Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Kalanthroff, Eyal; Goldfarb, Liat; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Performance of the Stroop task reflects two conflicts--informational (between the incongruent word and ink color) and task (between relevant color naming and irrelevant word reading). The task conflict is usually not visible, and is only seen when task control is damaged. Using the stop-signal paradigm, a few studies demonstrated longer…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Color, Naming, Word Recognition
Eitam, Baruch; Yeshurun, Yaffa; Hassan, Kinneret – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
To what degree does our representation of the immediate world depend solely on its relevance to what we are currently doing? We examined whether relevance per se can cause "blindness," even when there is no resource limitation. In a novel paradigm, people looked at a colored circle surrounded by a differently colored ring--the task…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Interference (Learning), Visual Perception, Sensory Experience
Cullipher, S.; Sevian, H.; Talanquer, V. – Chemistry Education Research and Practice, 2015
The ability to evaluate options and make informed decisions about problems in relevant contexts is a core competency in science education that requires the use of both domain-general and discipline-specific knowledge and reasoning strategies. In this study we investigated the implicit assumptions and modes of reasoning applied by individuals with…
Descriptors: Chemistry, Costs, Cost Effectiveness, Science Instruction
Collisson, Beverly Anne; Grela, Bernard; Spaulding, Tammie; Rueckl, Jay G.; Magnuson, James S. – Developmental Science, 2015
We investigated whether preschool children with specific language impairment (SLI) exhibit the shape bias in word learning: the bias to generalize based on shape rather than size, color, or texture in an object naming context ("This is a wek; find another wek") but not in a non-naming similarity classification context ("See this?…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Language Impairments, Bias, Geometric Concepts
Yoon, Jinah; Seo, Yeran; Kim, Jangjin; Lee, Inah – Learning & Memory, 2012
Cued retrieval of memory is typically examined with delay when testing hippocampal functions, as in delayed matching-to-sample tasks. Equally emphasized in the literature, on the other hand, is the hippocampal involvement in making arbitrary associations. Paired associate memory tasks are widely used for examining this function. However, the two…
Descriptors: Memory, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cues, Recall (Psychology)
Hemmer, Pernille; Criss, Amy H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
The role of experience in memory, specifically the word frequency (WF) mirror effect showing higher hit rates and lower false alarm rates for low-frequency words, is one of the hallmarks of memory. However, this "regularity of memory" is limited because normative WF has been treated as discrete (low vs. high). We evaluate the extent to…
Descriptors: Experience, Memory, Word Frequency, Experimental Psychology