NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 109 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jiang, Yuhong V.; Swallow, Khena M. – Cognition, 2013
Visual attention prioritizes information presented at particular spatial locations. These locations can be defined in reference frames centered on the environment or on the viewer. This study investigates whether incidentally learned attention uses a viewer-centered or environment-centered reference frame. Participants conducted visual search on a…
Descriptors: Attention Deficit Disorders, Attention, Probability, Incidental Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Spruyt, Adriaan; De Houwer, Jan; Everaert, Tom; Hermans, Dirk – Cognition, 2012
We examined whether semantic activation by subliminally presented stimuli is dependent upon the extent to which participants assign attention to specific semantic stimulus features and stimulus dimensions. Participants pronounced visible target words that were preceded by briefly presented, masked prime words. Both affective and non-affective…
Descriptors: Priming, Semantics, Attention Control, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sloutsky, Vladimir M.; Robinson, Christopher W. – Cognition, 2013
Many objects and events can be categorized in different ways, and learning multiple categories in parallel often requires flexibly attending to different stimulus dimensions in different contexts. Although infants and young children often exhibit poor attentional control, several theoretical proposals argue that such flexibility can be achieved…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Attention, Redundancy, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Drew, Trafton; Horowitz, Todd S.; Vogel, Edward K. – Cognition, 2013
In the multiple object tracking task, participants are asked to keep targets separate from identical distractors as all items move randomly. It is well known that simple manipulations such as object speed and number of distractors dramatically alter the number of targets that are successfully tracked, but very little is known about what "causes"…
Descriptors: Attention, Experiments, Spatial Ability, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frankenhuis, Willem E.; House, Bailey; Barrett, H. Clark; Johnson, Scott P. – Cognition, 2013
Two significant questions in cognitive and developmental science are first, whether objects and events are selected for attention based on their features (featural processing) or the configuration of their features (configural processing), and second, how these modes of processing develop. These questions have been addressed in part with…
Descriptors: Human Body, Infants, Effect Size, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brochard, Renaud; Tassin, Maxime; Zagar, Daniel – Cognition, 2013
The present research aimed to investigate whether, as previously observed with pictures, background auditory rhythm would also influence visual word recognition. In a lexical decision task, participants were presented with bisyllabic visual words, segmented into two successive groups of letters, while an irrelevant strongly metric auditory…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Language Processing, Auditory Stimuli, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Du, Feng; Abrams, Richard A. – Cognition, 2012
To avoid sensory overload, people are able to selectively attend to a particular color or direction of motion while ignoring irrelevant stimuli that differ from the desired one. We show here for the first time that it is also possible to selectively attend to a specific line orientation--but with an important caveat: orientations that are…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Motion, Stimuli, Neurology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Delvenne, Jean-Francois; Holt, Jessica L. – Cognition, 2012
Humans have the ability to attentionally select the most relevant visual information from their extrapersonal world and to retain it in a temporary buffer, known as visual short-term memory (VSTM). Research suggests that at least two non-contiguous items can be selected simultaneously when they are distributed across the two visual hemifields. In…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Infants, Attention, Cognitive Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Staudte, Maria; Crocker, Matthew W. – Cognition, 2011
Referential gaze during situated language production and comprehension is tightly coupled with the unfolding speech stream (Griffin, 2001; Meyer, Sleiderink, & Levelt, 1998; Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1995). In a shared environment, utterance comprehension may further be facilitated when the listener can exploit the speaker's…
Descriptors: Attention, Robotics, Nonverbal Communication, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dalton, Polly; Fraenkel, Nick – Cognition, 2012
It is now well-known that the absence of attention can leave us "blind" to visual stimuli that are very obvious under normal viewing conditions (e.g. a person dressed as a gorilla; Simons & Chabris, 1999). However, the question of whether hearing can ever be susceptible to such effects remains open. Here, we present evidence that the absence of…
Descriptors: Evidence, Visual Stimuli, Auditory Stimuli, Deafness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pincham, Hannah L.; Szucs, Denes – Cognition, 2012
Subitizing is traditionally described as the rapid, preattentive and automatic enumeration of up to four items. Counting, by contrast, describes the enumeration of larger sets of items and requires slower serial shifts of attention. Although recent research has called into question the preattentive nature of subitizing, whether or not numerosities…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Attention, Computation, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Benitez, Viridiana L.; Smith, Linda B. – Cognition, 2012
Expectancy-based localized attention has been shown to promote the formation and retrieval of multisensory memories in adults. Three experiments show that these processes also characterize attention and learning in 16- to 18-month old infants and, moreover, that these processes may play a critical role in supporting early object name learning. The…
Descriptors: Infants, Object Permanence, Prediction, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Risko, Evan F.; Anderson, Nicola C.; Lanthier, Sophie; Kingstone, Alan – Cognition, 2012
Visual exploration is driven by two main factors--the stimuli in our environment, and our own individual interests and intentions. Research investigating these two aspects of attentional guidance has focused almost exclusively on factors common across individuals. The present study took a different tack, and examined the role played by individual…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Personality Traits, Individual Differences, Intention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ariga, Atsunori; Lleras, Alejandro – Cognition, 2011
We newly propose that the vigilance decrement occurs because the cognitive control system fails to maintain active the goal of the vigilance task over prolonged periods of time (goal habituation). Further, we hypothesized that momentarily deactivating this goal (via a switch in tasks) would prevent the activation level of the vigilance goal from…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Brain, Task Analysis, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Magnani, Barbara; Pavani, Francesco; Frassinetti, Francesca – Cognition, 2012
The aim of the present study was to explore the spatial organization of auditory time and the effects of the manipulation of spatial attention on such a representation. In two experiments, we asked 28 adults to classify the duration of auditory stimuli as "short" or "long". Stimuli were tones of high or low pitch, delivered left or right of the…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Auditory Stimuli, Attention, Experiments
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8