Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 5 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 16 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 44 |
Descriptor
Attribution Theory | 102 |
Age Differences | 32 |
Children | 23 |
Preschool Children | 20 |
Social Cognition | 19 |
Cognitive Development | 18 |
Elementary Education | 18 |
Elementary School Students | 18 |
Inferences | 15 |
Adults | 13 |
Child Development | 13 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 102 |
Author
Graham, Sandra | 4 |
Beier, Jonathan S. | 2 |
Booth, Amy E. | 2 |
Bugental, Daphne Blunt | 2 |
Dweck, Carol S. | 2 |
Fabes, Richard A. | 2 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 2 |
Grusec, Joan E. | 2 |
Harris, Paul L. | 2 |
Heyman, Gail D. | 2 |
Kushnir, Tamar | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 97 |
Reports - Research | 92 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 5 |
Secondary Education | 3 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 9 |
Location
Canada | 5 |
Massachusetts | 2 |
Canada (Vancouver) | 1 |
District of Columbia | 1 |
Hong Kong | 1 |
Minnesota | 1 |
New York | 1 |
New Zealand | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
Singapore | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 2 |
Bracken Basic Concept Scale | 1 |
Matching Familiar Figures Test | 1 |
Multigroup Ethnic Identity… | 1 |
Social Skills Rating System | 1 |
Woodcock Johnson Psycho… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Tecwyn, Emma C.; Mazumder, Pingki; Buchsbaum, Daphna – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Knowing the temporal direction of causal relations is critical for producing desired outcomes and explaining events. Existing evidence suggests that children start to grasp that causes must precede their effects (the temporal priority principle) by age 3; however, whether younger children also understand this has, to our knowledge, not previously…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Time Perspective, Influences, Attribution Theory
Payir, Ayse; Heiphetz, Larisa; Harris, Paul L.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Recent research has shown that a religious upbringing renders children receptive to ordinarily impossible outcomes, but the underlying mechanism for this effect remains unclear. Exposure to religious teachings might alter children's basic understanding of causality. Alternatively, religious exposure might only affect children's religious…
Descriptors: Children, Religious Factors, Religious Education, Cognitive Development
Marshall, Julia; Gollwitzer, Anton; Bloom, Paul – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Past research has demonstrated that both consequentialist motives (such as deterrence) and deontological motives (such as "just deserts") underlie children's and adults' punitive behavior. But what motives do we ascribe to others who pursue punishment? The present work explores this question by assessing which punitive motives children…
Descriptors: Punishment, Behavior, Attribution Theory, Intention
Li, Pearl Han; Stephens Hoff, Elizabeth; Koenig, Melissa A. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
One developmental task faced by children is to identify, remember, and learn from epistemic and moral agents around them who are known to be good or virtuous. Here, in 2 studies, we examined U.S children's (N = 138; 55% female, 45% male; predominantly White, middle-class) memory processes for agents varying in moral and epistemic virtue. In Study…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Attribution Theory, Moral Values, Memory
Ding, Xiao Pan; Lim, Hui Yan; Heyman, Gail D. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Learning from others allows young children to acquire vast amounts of information quickly, but doing so effectively also requires epistemic vigilance. Although preschool-age children have some capacity to engage in such processes, they often have trouble resisting information from misleading informants. The present research takes a "novel…
Descriptors: Deception, Preschool Children, Recognition (Psychology), Task Analysis
Terrizzi, Brandon F.; Brey, Elizabeth; Shutts, Kristin; Beier, Jonathan S. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
When navigating unfamiliar social environments, it is important to identify who is powerful. Determining who has power can be challenging because observers may have limited social information, and because people achieve influence for many reasons. In experiments with 3- to 5-year-old children (n = 192) and adults (n = 32), we investigated the…
Descriptors: Power Structure, Young Children, Adults, Developmental Stages
Martin, Jodi; Anderson, Jacob E.; Groh, Ashley M.; Waters, Theodore E. A.; Young, Ethan; Johnson, William F.; Shankman, Jessica L.; Eller, Jami; Fleck, Cory; Steele, Ryan D.; Carlson, Elizabeth A.; Simpson, Jeffry A.; Roisman, Glenn I. – Developmental Psychology, 2018
This study examined the predictive significance of maternal sensitivity in early childhood for electrophysiological responding to and cognitive appraisals of infant crying at midlife in a sample of 73 adults (age = 39 years; 43 females; 58 parents) from the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation. When listening to an infant crying,…
Descriptors: Mothers, Young Children, Early Experience, Parent Child Relationship
LoBue, Vanessa; Adolph, Karen E. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
This review challenges the traditional interpretation of infants' and young children's responses to three types of potentially "fear-inducing" stimuli--snakes and spiders, heights, and strangers. The traditional account is that these stimuli are the objects of infants' earliest developing fears. We present evidence against the…
Descriptors: Fear, Emotional Response, Infants, Young Children
Kornbluh, Mariah E.; Pykett, Alisa A.; Flanagan, Constance A. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
This study explores whether adolescents' societal explanations regarding the causes of poverty are associated with distributive justice reasoning. Survey data were collected from 425 6th-12th graders who answered questions concerning the causes of poverty and a vignette depicting a hypothetical class project designed to assess the degree to which…
Descriptors: Adolescent Attitudes, Secondary School Students, Student Attitudes, Justice
Rakison, David H.; Smith, Gabriel Tobin; Ali, Areej – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Four experiments investigated infants' and adults' knowledge of the identity of objects in a causal sequence of events. In Experiments 1 and 2, 18- and 22-month-olds in the visual habituation procedure were shown a 3-step causal chain event in which the relation between an object's part (dynamic or static) and its causal role was either consistent…
Descriptors: Infants, Learning, Identification, Adults
Booth, Amy E.; Shavlik, Margaret; Haden, Catherine A. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
To explore the potential contribution of parents' causal talk to preschooler's emerging scientific literacy and related interests, we observed 153 parent-child dyads playing together in a museum and in the lab. As in previous work, the frequency with which parents referenced causal information in their speech predicted the strength of their…
Descriptors: Parent Child Relationship, Scientific Literacy, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Hershberg, Rachel M.; Johnson, Sara K. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
An intersectional approach to human development emphasizes the multiple social categories individuals occupy, some of which confer privilege (e.g., being White) and some of which confer marginalization (e.g., being poor). This approach is needed especially in critical consciousness scholarship, and particularly in regard to understanding whether…
Descriptors: Whites, Low Income, Working Class, Trade and Industrial Education
Rizzo, Michael T.; Li, Leon; Burkholder, Amanda R.; Killen, Melanie – Developmental Psychology, 2019
In a hidden inequality context, resource allocators and resource recipients are unaware that an unknowingly advantaged recipient possesses resources. The present study presented children aged 3-13 years (N = 121) with a hidden inequality vignette involving an accidental transgression in which one resource claimant, who unknowingly possessed more…
Descriptors: Deception, Child Development, Moral Values, Intention
Seider, Scott; Clark, Shelby; Graves, Daren; Kelly, Lauren Leigh; Soutter, Madora; El-Amin, Aaliyah; Jennett, Pauline – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Interpersonal and structural forms of racism contribute to a system of economic stratification in the United States in which children of color are disproportionately likely to be born into poverty and to remain poor as adults. However, only a small body of research has focused on Black and Latinx adolescents' developing beliefs about the causes of…
Descriptors: Hispanic American Students, Poverty, Racial Bias, Social Differences
Ma, Lili; Xu, Fei – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Human adults have a strong bias to invoke intentional agents in their intuitive explanations of ordered wholes or regular compositions in the world. Less is known about the ontogenetic origin of this bias. In 4 experiments, we found that 9-to 10-month-old infants expected a human hand, but not a mechanical tool with similar affordances, to be the…
Descriptors: Infants, Inferences, Bias, Expectation