Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Bias | 5 |
Classification | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Attention | 2 |
Beliefs | 2 |
Child Development | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
Language Acquisition | 2 |
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) | 2 |
Toddlers | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Diesendruck, Gil | 5 |
Bloom, Paul | 2 |
Benozio, Avi | 1 |
Ferera, Matar | 1 |
Gelman, Susan | 1 |
Goldfein-Elbaz, Rebecca | 1 |
Graham, Susan A. | 1 |
Markson, Lori | 1 |
Neumark, Noam | 1 |
Rhodes, Marjorie | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 5 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Israel | 1 |
North America | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ferera, Matar; Benozio, Avi; Diesendruck, Gil – Child Development, 2020
Adults' attraction to rare objects has been variously attributed to fundamental biases related to resource availability, self-related needs, or beliefs about social and market forces. The current three studies investigated the scarcity bias in 11- and 14-month-old infants, and 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 129). With slight methodological…
Descriptors: Attention, Bias, Infants, Young Children
Diesendruck, Gil; Goldfein-Elbaz, Rebecca; Rhodes, Marjorie; Gelman, Susan; Neumark, Noam – Child Development, 2013
The present study compared 5-and 10-year-old North American and Israeli children's beliefs about the objectivity of different categories (n = 109). Children saw picture triads composed of two exemplars of the same category (e.g., two women) and an exemplar of a contrasting category (e.g., a man). Children were asked whether it would be acceptable…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Cultural Differences, Comparative Analysis, Children
Graham, Susan A.; Diesendruck, Gil – Cognitive Development, 2010
This study examined whether infants privilege shape over other perceptual properties when making inferences about the shared properties of novel objects. Forty-six 15-month-olds were presented with novel target objects that possessed a nonobvious property, followed by test objects that varied in shape, color, or texture relative to the target.…
Descriptors: Infants, Perception, Inferences, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Markson, Lori; Diesendruck, Gil; Bloom, Paul – Developmental Science, 2008
When children learn the name of a novel object, they tend to extend that name to other objects similar in shape--a phenomenon referred to as the shape bias. Does the shape bias stem from learned associations between names and categories of objects, or does it derive from more general properties of children's understanding of language and the…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Bias, Geometric Concepts

Diesendruck, Gil; Bloom, Paul – Child Development, 2003
Three studies explored whether children's tendency to extend object names on the basis of sameness of shape (shape bias) is specific to naming. Findings indicated that 2- and 3-year-olds showed shape bias both when asked to extend a novel name and when asked to select an object of the same kind as a target object; 3-year-olds also showed shape…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Beliefs, Bias, Classification