Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Bilingualism | 6 |
Sign Language | 4 |
American Sign Language | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Deafness | 2 |
English | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Language Research | 2 |
Nonverbal Communication | 2 |
Oral Language | 2 |
Second Language Instruction | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Bilingualism: Language and… | 6 |
Author
Emmorey, Karen | 3 |
Borinstein, Helsa B. | 1 |
Capirci, Olga | 1 |
Casey, Shannon | 1 |
Gollan, Tamar H. | 1 |
Iverson, Jana M. | 1 |
Larrabee, Heather | 1 |
Montanari, Sandro | 1 |
Morgan, Gary | 1 |
Nicodemus, Brenda | 1 |
Thompson, Robin | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Netherlands | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Nicodemus, Brenda; Emmorey, Karen – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2013
Spoken language (unimodal) interpreters often prefer to interpret from their non-dominant language (L2) into their native language (L1). Anecdotally, signed language (bimodal) interpreters express the opposite bias, preferring to interpret from L1 (spoken language) into L2 (signed language). We conducted a large survey study ("N" =…
Descriptors: Deaf Interpreting, Sign Language, Native Language, Second Languages
Woll, Bencie; Morgan, Gary – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2012
Various theories of developmental language impairments have sought to explain these impairments in modality-specific ways--for example, that the language deficits in SLI or Down syndrome arise from impairments in auditory processing. Studies of signers with language impairments, especially those who are bilingual in a spoken language as well as a…
Descriptors: Speech, Oral Language, Language Impairments, Down Syndrome
van Beijsterveldt, Liesbeth M.; van Hell, Janet G. – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2012
This study examined temporal reference marking in texts written by Dutch deaf children and adults who differed in sign language proficiency. The temporal reference marking systems in Dutch and Sign Language of the Netherlands (SLN) differ substantially, with Dutch having a wide range of lexical and morphological markers of temporal reference, and…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Sign Language, Deafness
Casey, Shannon; Emmorey, Karen; Larrabee, Heather – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2012
Given that the linguistic articulators for sign language are also used to produce co-speech gesture, we examined whether one year of academic instruction in American Sign Language (ASL) impacts the rate and nature of gestures produced when speaking English. A survey study revealed that 75% of ASL learners (N = 95), but only 14% of Romance language…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, American Sign Language, Cartoons, Second Language Learning
Emmorey, Karen; Borinstein, Helsa B.; Thompson, Robin; Gollan, Tamar H. – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2008
Speech-sign or "bimodal" bilingualism is exceptional because distinct modalities allow for simultaneous production of two languages. We investigated the ramifications of this phenomenon for models of language production by eliciting language mixing from eleven hearing native users of American Sign Language (ASL) and English. Instead of switching…
Descriptors: Semantics, American Sign Language, Bilingualism, Oral Language

Capirci, Olga; Iverson, Jana M.; Montanari, Sandro; Volterra, Virginia – Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2002
Examined potential effects of early exposure to sign language on the use of communicative gestures by a bilingual hearing child of deaf parents. Data were analyzed to identify types and tokens of communicative gestures, words, signs, and the ways in which they were combined. (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Deafness, Foreign Countries, Language Acquisition