Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 20 |
Cognitive Processes | 20 |
Infant Behavior | 20 |
Infants | 12 |
Cognitive Development | 8 |
Toddlers | 4 |
Learning Processes | 3 |
Parent Child Relationship | 3 |
Perception | 3 |
Theories | 3 |
Behavior Patterns | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Adolph, Karen E. | 1 |
Berger, Sarah E. | 1 |
Biro, Szilvia | 1 |
Brandon M. Woo | 1 |
Buckley, Marilyn | 1 |
Cohen, Leslie B. | 1 |
Del Giudice, Marco | 1 |
Elizabeth S. Spelke | 1 |
Fava, Eswen | 1 |
Feigenson, Lisa | 1 |
Haith, Marshall, M. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Opinion Papers | 3 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Books | 1 |
Collected Works - General | 1 |
Guides - Non-Classroom | 1 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 5 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stanford Binet Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brandon M. Woo; Shari Liu; Elizabeth S. Spelke – Developmental Science, 2024
Does knowledge of other people's minds grow from concrete experience to abstract concepts? Cognitive scientists have hypothesized that infants' first-person experience, acting on their own goals, leads them to understand others' actions and goals. Indeed, classic developmental research suggests that before infants reach for objects, they do not…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Inferences, Infant Behavior
Hirai, Masahiro; Kanakogi, Yasuhiro – Developmental Science, 2019
The theory of natural pedagogy has proposed that infants can use ostensive signals, including eye contact, infant-directed speech, and contingency to learn from others. However, the role of bodily gestures, such as hand-waving, in social learning has been largely ignored. To address this gap in the literature, this study sought to determine…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Teaching Methods, Infants, Infant Behavior
Pickron, Charisse B.; Iyer, Arjun; Fava, Eswen; Scott, Lisa S. – Child Development, 2018
This study examined differences in visual attention as a function of label learning from 6 to 9 months of age. Before and after 3 months of parent-directed storybook training with computer-generated novel objects, event-related potentials and visual fixations were recorded while infants viewed trained and untrained images (n = 23). Relative to a…
Descriptors: Child Development, Visual Perception, Attention Control, Parent Child Relationship
Verschoor, Stephan A.; Spapé, Michiel; Biro, Szilvia; Hommel, Bernhard – Developmental Science, 2013
Ideomotor theory considers bidirectional action-effect associations to be the fundamental building blocks for intentional action. The present study employed a novel pupillometric and oculomotor paradigm to study developmental changes in the role of action-effects in the acquisition of voluntary action. Our findings suggest that both 7- and…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Cognitive Processes, Prediction
Morrissey, Anne-Marie – Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2014
As part of a longitudinal study, infant/toddler pretend play development and maternal play modelling were investigated in dyadic context. A total of 21 children were videotaped in monthly play sessions with their mothers, from age 8 to 17 months. Child and mother pretend play frequencies and levels were measured using Brown's Pretend Play…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Toddlers, Mothers, Play
Moher, Mariko; Tuerk, Arin S.; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Although working memory has a highly constrained capacity limit of three or four items, both adults and toddlers can increase the total amount of stored information by "chunking" object representations in memory. To examine the developmental origins of chunking, we used a violation-of-expectation procedure to ask whether 7-month-old infants, whose…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Infants, Short Term Memory
Trauble, Birgit; Marinovic, Vesna; Pauen, Sabina – Infancy, 2010
Recent studies suggest that even infants attend to others' beliefs in order to make sense of their behavior. To warrant the assumption of early belief understanding, corresponding competences need to be demonstrated in a variety of different belief-inducing situations. The present study provides corresponding evidence, using a completely nonverbal…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Competence
Troseth, Georgene L. – Developmental Review, 2010
This paper offers an overview of research on infants' early behavior toward televised images, followed by an account of the development of "representational competence" with video. Several aspects of representation are involved in young children's understanding and use of video. From a very young age, children form mental representations of the…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Infants, Television Viewing, Behavior Patterns
Del Giudice, Marco; Manera, Valeria; Keysers, Christian – Developmental Science, 2009
Mirror neurons are increasingly recognized as a crucial substrate for many developmental processes, including imitation and social learning. Although there has been considerable progress in describing their function and localization in the primate and adult human brain, we still know little about their ontogeny. The idea that mirror neurons result…
Descriptors: Socialization, Student Attitudes, Brain, Children

Haith, Marshall, M. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1972
Revised version of paper read at the American Psychological Association meetings at Miami Beach in September, 1970. Presented at a symposium on The Meaning of Smiling and Vocalizing in Infancy.'' (CB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior

Buckley, Marilyn – Theory into Practice, 1979
When children enter school, their principal way of thinking is through talking aloud (rather than through internal verbalization), and they need to be encouraged in their use of language as they confront new academic problems. (JD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior, Language

Kagan, Jerome – Science, 1970
Describes the reaction of infants and their attention to events by actions such as vocalizing, smiling or a change in respiration rate. Suggests that understanding of infant selectivity and duration of attention should provide insight into the nature of psychological growth. (JM)
Descriptors: Behavior Theories, Child Development, Child Psychology, Cognitive Processes
Berger, Sarah E.; Adolph, Karen E.; Lobo, Sharon A. – Child Development, 2005
This study examined whether 16-month-old walking infants take the material composition of a handrail into account when assessing its effectiveness as a tool to augment balance. Infants were encouraged to cross from one platform to another via bridges of various widths (10, 20, 40cm) with either a wobbly (foam or latex) or a wooden handrail…
Descriptors: Child Development, Physical Activities, Infant Behavior, Toddlers
Urban, Elizabeth – Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 2006
This paper is a response to a review of the conference titled, "Unintegration, Disintegration and Integration", written by Cathy Urwin and Maria Rhode in the ACP Bulletin. The review mentioned Michael Fordham, noting that he referred to a "good" kind of unintegration. In this paper, I point out that this is a somewhat misleading reference to what…
Descriptors: Models, Infants, Child Development, Developmental Stages

Wishart, Jennifer G. – Child Development, 1986
Investigates whether 6- to 12-month-old infants' exposure to the successful search behavior of a sibling in two object-concept tasks would enhance infants' subsequent performance on these tasks. (HOD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2