Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 32 |
Symbolic Learning | 32 |
Cognitive Development | 13 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Learning Processes | 6 |
Play | 6 |
Children | 5 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Young Children | 5 |
Art Education | 4 |
Associative Learning | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Overton, Willis F. | 2 |
Athey, Irene | 1 |
Ball, Samuel | 1 |
Ball, Steven | 1 |
Bates, Elizabeth | 1 |
Bialystok, Ellen | 1 |
Bogatz, Gerry Ann | 1 |
Bower, B. | 1 |
Bruce, Susan M. | 1 |
Bruner, Jerome S. | 1 |
Casby, Michael W. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 17 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Reports - Descriptive | 4 |
Information Analyses | 3 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 4 |
Parents | 2 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lee, Gabrielle T.; Xu, Sheng; Guo, Shufang; Gilic, Lina; Pu, Yunhuan; Xu, Jiacheng – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2019
Symbolic play skills are important in language acquisition and child development. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have difficulties demonstrating such play behaviors. Imaginary objects symbolic play refers to play behavior in which children perform play actions without actual objects. Three boys with ASD (3-7 years) participated…
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Imagination, Play, Symbolic Learning
Zambrana, Imac M.; Ystrom, Eivind; Schjolberg, Synnve; Pons, Francisco – Child Development, 2013
This study examined whether poor pointing gestures and imitative actions at 18 months of age uniquely predicted late language production at 36 months, beyond the role of poor language at 18 months of age. Data from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study were utilized. Maternal reports of the children's nonverbal skills and language were…
Descriptors: Imitation, Toddlers, Prediction, Child Development
Gelman, Susan A.; Waxman, Sandra R.; Kleinberg, Felicia – Cognitive Development, 2008
Mother-child conversations about pictures systematically differ from mother-child conversations about objects: Pictures are more likely than objects to elicit talk about kinds, whereas objects are more likely than pictures to elicit talk about individuals. The purpose of the current study is to examine whether this difference between pictures and…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Mothers, Concept Formation, Child Development

Rogers, Sally J.; Puchalski, Carol B. – Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 1984
The beginnings of representational play were examined in 16 visually impaired children (18-38 months). Nine children demonstrated some symbolic acts at a mean age significantly earlier than the literature suggests. Presence of symbolic acts was significantly related to use of the word "no," two-word combinations, and general sensorimotor skills.…
Descriptors: Child Development, Developmental Stages, Infants, Play

Lombardino, Linda J.; And Others – Journal of Communication Disorders, 1986
Structural play-language relationships and diversity of symbolic play skills were compared with five language impaired and five nonimpaired children (ages 27-39 months). Findings indicated differences in mean length of utterance, mean length of sequence, and quantitative and qualitative aspects of symbolic play. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Language Acquisition, Language Handicaps, Play

Thal, Donna; Bates, Elizabeth – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1988
The relationship between language and symbolic gesture was studied in nine children (ages 18-32 months) who were in the lowest 10 percent for language production for their age. Subjects performed like language-matched controls on a lexical production task. On a syntax-related task, they performed like age-matched and significantly better than…
Descriptors: Child Development, Delayed Speech, Language Acquisition, Nonverbal Communication
Farnham-Diggory, Sylvia – Monogr Soc Res Child Develop, 1970
Three experiments explore factors associated with the development of certain symbolic abilities (verbal, maplike, and mathematical) in 4-10 year old Negro and white children. The experimental tasks required a synthesis of concepts with symbolic stimuli. (MH)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Racial Differences, Sensory Training

Bialystok, Ellen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2000
Two studies examined understanding of notational representation among 3- to 5-year-olds. Children solved problems when shown cards with a picture or word indicating identity or a quantity indicator. In the easier condition, children had difficulty solving the problems as a function of their familiarity with the notation, suggesting weaknesses in…
Descriptors: Child Development, Coding, Cognitive Development, Pattern Recognition

Overton, Willis F.; Jackson, Joseph P. – Child Development, 1973
Major finding of this study demonstrates clear support for the proposed developmental sequence of gestural representation. (Authors)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Developmental Psychology, Imagination

Nowak-Fabrykowski, Krystyna – Journal of Creative Behavior, 1992
This paper discusses links among the process of creativity, symbolization, and learning. The importance of symbolization in thinking, in school learning, in child development, and in the behavior of creative learners is stressed. (DB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Processes, Creative Development
Siegler, Robert S. – American Psychologist, 2005
A new field of children's learning is emerging. This new field differs from the old in recognizing that children's learning includes active as well as passive mechanisms and qualitative as well as quantitative changes. Children's learning involves substantial variability of representations and strategies within individual children as well as…
Descriptors: Children, Active Learning, Learning Strategies, Models

Steiner, Gerhard – Child Development, 1974
Piaget's concept of "internalization" is analyzed and related to Bruner's representational theories of cognitive development. Cognitive structures are shown to be real exclusively in an actual representation. (Author/SDH)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis
Athey, Irene – 1984
The review surveys theories and models of human development and extrapolates from these to form a composite sketch of the "typical" deaf adolescent. Theories of J. Piaget are examined and the major stages of cognitive development he proposed are summarized. Implications of concepts of disequilibrium and restructuring of cognitive schemas…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Deafness

Casby, Michael W. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 1997
This paper reviews research on symbolic play gaps between children with and without language impairment (LI), arguing that these gaps should not be interpreted as demonstrative of marked deficits in general representational or specific symbolic play competence of children with LI. Frequent encroachment of language into testing procedures is cited…
Descriptors: Child Development, Language Impairments, Play, Pretend Play
Pelz, Ruth, Ed. – 1982
Five papers are presented from a 1982 conference on "Developmental and Clinical Aspects of Young Children's Play." In the first paper ("Cognitive Characteristics of Young Children's Play,") S. Rogers summarizes J. Piaget's theories on developmental stages, discusses the relationship of practice play to learning, and then…
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Disabilities, High Risk Persons