Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 6 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 15 |
Descriptor
Child Language | 49 |
Sign Language | 49 |
Language Acquisition | 35 |
Deafness | 28 |
Language Research | 14 |
Parent Child Relationship | 12 |
Foreign Countries | 11 |
Bilingualism | 10 |
Nonverbal Communication | 10 |
Infants | 8 |
Semantics | 8 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Location
Japan | 2 |
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
United States | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Belgium | 1 |
Brazil | 1 |
China | 1 |
France | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
South Carolina | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Preschool Language Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Tilbe Göksun; Asli Aktan-Erciyes; Dilay Z. Karadöller; Ö. Ece Demir-Lira – Child Development Perspectives, 2025
Children need to learn the demands of their native language in the early vocabulary development phase. In this dynamic process, parental multimodal input may shape neurodevelopmental trajectories while also being tailored by child-related factors. Moving beyond typically characterized group profiles, in this article, we synthesize growing evidence…
Descriptors: Parent Participation, Parent Child Relationship, Child Language, Vocabulary Development
Perniss, Pamela; Lu, Jenny C.; Morgan, Gary; Vigliocco, Gabriella – Developmental Science, 2018
Most research on the mechanisms underlying referential mapping has assumed that learning occurs in ostensive contexts, where label and referent co-occur, and that form and meaning are linked by arbitrary convention alone. In the present study, we focus on "iconicity" in language, that is, resemblance relationships between form and…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Linguistic Input, Child Language, Semiotics
Green, Jennifer; Hodge, Gabrielle; Kelly, Barbara F. – Language Documentation & Conservation, 2022
In this article, we provide an overview of the last twenty years of research on Indigenous sign languages, deaf community sign languages, co-speech gesture, and multimodal communication in the Australian context. From a global perspective, research on sign languages and on the gestures that normally accompany speech has been used as the basis for…
Descriptors: Deafness, Indigenous Populations, Sign Language, Nonverbal Communication
Pernis, Pamela; Lu, Jenny C.; Morgan, Gary; Vigliocco, Gabriella – Grantee Submission, 2017
Most research on the mechanisms underlying referential mapping has assumed that learning occurs in ostensive contexts, where label and referent co-occur, and that form and meaning are linked by arbitrary convention alone. In the present study, we focus on "iconicity" in language, i.e. resemblance relationships between form and meaning,…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Sign Language, Language Acquisition, Semiotics
Ozyurek, Asli; Furman, Reyhan; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Journal of Child Language, 2015
Languages typically express semantic components of motion events such as manner (roll) and path (down) in separate lexical items. We explore how these combinatorial possibilities of language arise by focusing on (i) gestures produced by deaf children who lack access to input from a conventional language (homesign); (ii) gestures produced by…
Descriptors: Child Language, Nonverbal Communication, Semantics, Deafness
Lu, Jenny; Jones, Anna; Morgan, Gary – Journal of Child Language, 2016
There is debate about how input variation influences child language. Most deaf children are exposed to a sign language from their non-fluent hearing parents and experience a delay in exposure to accessible language. A small number of children receive language input from their deaf parents who are fluent signers. Thus it is possible to document the…
Descriptors: Child Language, Sign Language, Deafness, Language Acquisition
Pichler, Deborah Chen; Hochgesang, Julie A.; Lillo-Martin, Diane; de Quadros, Ronice Müller; Reynolds, Wanette – Sign Language Studies, 2016
This article addresses the special challenges associated with collecting longitudinal samples of the spontaneous sign language and spoken language production by young bimodal bilingual children. We discuss the methods used in our study of children in the United States and Brazil. Since one of our goals is to observe both sign language and speech,…
Descriptors: Best Practices, Sign Language, Longitudinal Studies, Bilingualism
Hochgesang, Julie A. – ProQuest LLC, 2013
In my dissertation, I examine four notation systems used to represent hand configurations in child acquisition of signed languages. Linguists have long recognized the descriptive limitations of Stokoe notation, currently the most commonly used system for phonetic or phonological transcription, but continue using it because of its widespread…
Descriptors: Child Language, Sign Language, Phonetics, Phonology
Kirk, Elizabeth; Howlett, Neil; Pine, Karen J.; Fletcher, Ben C. – Child Development, 2013
Findings are presented from the first randomized control trial of the effects of encouraging symbolic gesture (or "baby sign") on infant language, following 40 infants from age 8 months to 20 months. Half of the mothers were trained to model a target set of gestures to their infants. Frequent measures were taken of infant language…
Descriptors: Infants, Sign Language, Language Acquisition, Child Language
Fitzpatrick, Elizabeth M.; Thibert, Jonelle; Grandpierre, Viviane; Johnston, J. Cyne – First Language, 2014
Baby sign language is advocated to improve children's communication development. However, the evidence to support the advantages of baby sign has been inconclusive. A systematic review was undertaken to summarize and appraise the research related to the effectiveness of symbolic gestures for typically developing, hearing infants with hearing…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Child Language, Nonverbal Communication, Infants
Brereton, Amy Elizabeth – Young Children, 2010
Infants' hands are ready to construct words using sign language before their mouths are ready to speak. These research findings may explain the popularity of parents and caregivers teaching and using sign language with infants and toddlers, along with speech. The advantages of using sign language with young children go beyond the infant and…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Interpersonal Communication, Preschool Children, Preschool Teachers
Surian, Luca; Tedoldi, Mariantonia; Siegel, Michael – Journal of Child Language, 2010
We investigated whether access to a sign language affects the development of pragmatic competence in three groups of deaf children aged 6 to 11 years: native signers from deaf families receiving bimodal/bilingual instruction, native signers from deaf families receiving oralist instruction and late signers from hearing families receiving oralist…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Deafness, Pragmatics, Bilingualism
Pettenati, Paola; Stefanini, Silvia; Volterra, Virginia – Journal of Child Language, 2010
This study explores the form of representational gestures produced by forty-five hearing children (age range 2 ; 0-3 ; 1) asked to label pictures in words. Five pictures depicting objects and five pictures depicting actions which elicited more representational gestures were chosen for more detailed analysis. The range of gestures produced for each…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Child Language, Young Children, Pictorial Stimuli
Barnes, Susan Kubic – Dimensions of Early Childhood, 2010
Teaching sign language--to deaf or other children with special needs or to hearing children with hard-of-hearing family members--is not new. Teaching sign language to typically developing children has become increasingly popular since the publication of "Baby Signs"[R] (Goodwyn & Acredolo, 1996), now in its third edition. Attention to signing with…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Special Needs Students, Language Acquisition, Hearing Impairments
Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Mylander, Carolyn; Franklin, Amy – Cognitive Psychology, 2007
When children learn language, they apply their language-learning skills to the linguistic input they receive. But what happens if children are not exposed to input from a conventional language? Do they engage their language-learning skills nonetheless, applying them to whatever unconventional input they have? We address this question by examining…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Linguistic Input, Sign Language, Deafness