Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 41 |
Descriptor
Child Psychology | 83 |
Cognitive Processes | 83 |
Child Development | 25 |
Children | 19 |
Cognitive Development | 15 |
Experimental Psychology | 15 |
Age Differences | 10 |
Comparative Analysis | 10 |
Developmental Psychology | 10 |
Memory | 10 |
Young Children | 10 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Elkind, David | 2 |
Hall, William S. | 2 |
Maurer, Daphne | 2 |
Nigg, Joel T. | 2 |
Alderson, R. Matt | 1 |
Andrews, Glenda | 1 |
Anthony, W. S. | 1 |
Anzures, Gizelle | 1 |
Arnold, L. Eugene | 1 |
Ashcraft, Mark H. | 1 |
Baddeley, Alan D. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 51 |
Reports - Research | 40 |
Information Analyses | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Books | 3 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Collected Works - General | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 10 |
Elementary Education | 10 |
Adult Education | 2 |
Grade 2 | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Grade 8 | 1 |
Grade 9 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
State Trait Anxiety Inventory… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Halpern, Mark – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2016
A new solution is offered to the Infant Language Acquisition Problem, rejecting both of Chomsky's alternatives. It proposes that the infant does not acquire his mother tongue by mastering its grammar, whether by inference from personal experience or via an innate Language Acquisition Device such as the UG, but that the language he hears is all…
Descriptors: Native Language Instruction, Native Speakers, Language Acquisition, Infants
vanMarle, Kristy – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Previous research has shown indirectly that infants may use two different mechanisms-an object tracking system and an analog magnitude mechanism--to represent small (less than 4) and large (greater than or equal to 4) numbers of objects, respectively. The current study directly tested this hypothesis in an ordinal choice task by presenting 10- to…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Psychology, Decision Making, Cognitive Processes
Sakkalou, Elena; Ellis-Davies, Kate; Fowler, Nia C.; Hilbrink, Elma E.; Gattis, Merideth – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Previous studies have reported that infants selectively reproduce observed actions and have argued that this selectivity reflects understanding of intentions and goals, or goal-directed imitation. We reasoned that if selective imitation of goal-directed actions reflects understanding of intentions, infants should demonstrate stability across…
Descriptors: Imitation, Infants, Goal Orientation, Experimental Psychology
Peets, Katlin; Hodges, Ernest V. E.; Salmivalli, Christina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Children and adolescents encounter different hurtful experiences in school settings. How these events are processed (e.g., whether they think that the transgressor was hostile) is likely to depend on the relationship with the transgressor. In this study, we examined how adolescents (58 girls and 35 boys, mean age = 14.03 years, SD = 0.60) dealt…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Psychological Patterns, Adolescents, Aggression
Chevalier, Nicolas; Wiebe, Sandra A.; Huber, Kristina L.; Espy, Kimberly Andrews – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The current study addressed the role of switch detection in cognitive flexibility by testing the effect of transition cues (i.e., cues that directly signal the need to switch or maintain a given task goal) in a cued set-shifting paradigm at 5 years of age. Children performed better, especially on switch trials, when transition cues were combined…
Descriptors: Cues, Young Children, Cognitive Processes, Experimental Psychology
Legare, Cristine H.; Mills, Candice M.; Souza, Andre L.; Plummer, Leigh E.; Yasskin, Rebecca – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
This study examined the strategic use of questions to solve problems across early childhood. Participants (N = 54, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds) engaged in two tasks: a novel problem-solving question task that required asking questions to an informant to determine which card in an array was located in a box and a cognitive flexibility task that…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Questioning Techniques, Young Children, Age Differences
Anzures, Gizelle; Wheeler, Andrea; Quinn, Paul C.; Pascalis, Olivier; Slater, Alan M.; Heron-Delaney, Michelle; Tanaka, James W.; Lee, Kang – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Perceptual narrowing in the visual, auditory, and multisensory domains has its developmental origins during infancy. The current study shows that experimentally induced experience can reverse the effects of perceptual narrowing on infants' visual recognition memory of other-race faces. Caucasian 8- to 10-month-olds who could not discriminate…
Descriptors: Females, Infants, Recognition (Psychology), Whites
Andrews, Glenda; Halford, Graeme S.; Boyce, Jillian – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Two experiments examined conditional discrimination in 4- to 6-year-olds. Children learned to choose one of two objects (e.g., circle) when the background was, say, red and to choose the other object (e.g., triangle) when the background was, say, blue. Awareness was assessed and interpreted as a marker of relational processing. In Experiment 1,…
Descriptors: Evaluation Methods, Geometric Concepts, Children, Age Differences
Schepman, Karen; Taylor, Eric; Collishaw, Stephan; Fombonne, Eric – Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 2012
Studies of adults with depression point to characteristic neurocognitive deficits, including differences in processing facial expressions. Few studies have examined face processing in juvenile depression, or taken account of other comorbid disorders. Three groups were compared: depressed children and adolescents with conduct disorder (n = 23),…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Adolescents, Depression (Psychology), Cognitive Processes
In-Albon, Tina; Schneider, Silvia – Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 2012
Cognitive biases are of interest in understanding the development of anxiety disorders. They also play a significant role during psychotherapy, where cognitive biases are modified in order to break the vicious cycle responsible for maintaining anxiety disorders. In a previous study, the vigilance-avoidance pattern was shown in children with…
Descriptors: Anxiety Disorders, Child Psychology, Psychotherapy, Separation Anxiety
Ashcraft, Mark H.; Moore, Alex M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We tested children in Grades 1 to 5, as well as college students, on a number line estimation task and examined latencies and errors to explore the cognitive processes involved in estimation. The developmental trends in estimation were more consistent with the hypothesized shift from logarithmic to linear representation than with an account based…
Descriptors: Mathematics Tests, Cognitive Processes, Standardized Tests, Computation
Liberman, Nira; Polack, Orli; Hameiri, Boaz; Blumenfeld, Maayan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
According to construal level theory, psychological distance promotes more abstract thought. Theories of creativity, in turn, suggest that abstract thought promotes creativity. Based on these lines of theorizing, we predicted that spatial distancing would enhance creative performance in elementary school children. To test this prediction, we primed…
Descriptors: Priming, Elementary School Students, Creativity, Creativity Tests
Brito, Natalie; Barr, Rachel; McIntyre, Paula; Simcock, Gabrielle – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Television viewing and picture book reading are prevalent activities during toddlerhood, and research has shown that toddlers can imitate from both books and videos after short delays. This is the first study to directly compare toddlers' long-term retention rates for target actions learned from a video or book. Toddlers (N = 158) at 18- and…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Television Viewing, Picture Books, Transfer of Training
Wilson, Ruth A. – Exchange: The Early Childhood Leaders' Magazine Since 1978, 2010
Rachel Carson (1956)--scientist, writer, and environmentalist--states that "A child's world is fresh and new and beautiful, full of wonder and excitement". Many people have heard and been inspired by these words, but may not have a clear idea about what wonder really is. This isn't surprising, because wonder in different contexts can mean…
Descriptors: Young Children, Child Development, Aesthetics, Fantasy
Schum, Nina; Franz, Volker H.; Jovanovic, Bianca; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We investigated whether 6- and 7-year-olds and 9- and 10-year-olds, as well as adults, process object dimensions independent of or in interaction with one another in a perception and action task by adapting Ganel and Goodale's method for testing adults ("Nature", 2003, Vol. 426, pp. 664-667). In addition, we aimed to confirm Ganel and Goodale's…
Descriptors: Evidence, Handicrafts, Visual Perception, Interaction