NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 24 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Armitage, Kristy L.; Redshaw, Jonathan – Child Development, 2022
Ninety-seven children aged 4-11 (49 males, 48 females, mostly White) were given the opportunity to improve their problem-solving performance by devising and implementing a novel cognitive offloading strategy. Across two phases, they searched for hidden rewards using maps that were either aligned or misaligned with the search space. In the second…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Style, Cognitive Processes, Problem Solving
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Burles, Ford; Liu, Irene; Hart, Chelsie; Murias, Kara; Graham, Susan A.; Iaria, Giuseppe – Child Development, 2020
Although much is known about adults' ability to orient by means of cognitive maps (mental representations of the environment), it is less clear when this important ability emerges in development. In the present study, 97 seven- to 10-year-olds and 26 adults played a video game designed to investigate the ability to orient using cognitive maps. The…
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Spatial Ability, Children, Navigation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Boccia, Maddalena; Vecchione, Francesca; Di Vita, Antonella; D'Amico, Simonetta; Guariglia, Cecilia; Piccardi, Laura – Child Development, 2019
Notwithstanding its well-established role on high-demanding spatial navigation tasks during adulthood, the effect of field dependence-independence during the acquisition of spatial navigation skills is almost unknown. This study assessed for the first time the effect of field dependence-independence on topographical learning (TL) across the life…
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Spatial Ability, Role, Navigation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Liben, Lynn S.; Myers, Lauren J.; Christensen, Adam E.; Bower, Corinne A. – Child Development, 2013
Researchers have shown that young children solve mapping tasks in small spaces, but have rarely tested children's performance in large, unfamiliar environments. In the current research, children (9-10 years; N = 40) explored an unfamiliar campus and marked flags' locations on a map. As hypothesized, better performance was predicted by…
Descriptors: Children, Map Skills, Spatial Ability, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vukovic, Rose K.; Fuchs, Lynn S.; Geary, David C.; Jordan, Nancy C.; Gersten, Russell; Siegler, Robert S. – Child Development, 2014
Longitudinal associations of domain-general and numerical competencies with individual differences in children's understanding of fractions were investigated. Children (n = 163) were assessed at 6 years of age on domain-general (nonverbal reasoning, language, attentive behavior, executive control, visual-spatial memory) and numerical (number…
Descriptors: Children, Individual Differences, Mathematics, Arithmetic
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Richmond, P. G. – Child Development, 1980
Sex pencil-and-paper spatial tests were administered to 232 boys and 237 girls with an average age of 10 years. Results suggest that sex differences in spatial ability may emerge before adolescence, but they are not necessarily generalized at that time. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Children, Sex Differences, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Leijenhorst, Linda; Crone, Eveline A.; Van der Molen, Maurits W. – Child Development, 2007
This study examined developmental trends in object and spatial working memory (WM) using heart rate (HR) to provide an index of covert cognitive processes. Participants in 4 age groups (6-7, 9-10, 11-12, 18-26, n=20 each) performed object and spatial WM tasks, in which each trial was followed by feedback. Spatial WM task performance reached adult…
Descriptors: Memory, Feedback, Children, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Dean, Anne L.; Deist, Steven – Child Development, 1980
The processes by which children construct images of anticipated end states of a transposition movement were examined on two tasks. Results support Piaget's (1977) hypothesis that reasoning on the basis of state correspondence defines a developmental level which precedes the development of transformational thought. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Processes, Conservation (Concept), Imagery
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Haake, Robert J.; And Others – Child Development, 1980
Investigated the ability of preschool and early grade school children to use logical search strategies to find a missing object on a familiar playground. Results indicated that children of all ages studied could deduce a critical search area and were not guided primarily by strong spatial associations. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Children, Logical Thinking, Preschool Children, Problem Solving
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
And Others; Vasta, Ross – Child Development, 1980
Accuracy of pattern copying was studied in male and female 10-year-olds. Contrary to expectations, independent of the stimulus size, males benefited from spatial response cues whereas females did not. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Children, Psychomotor Skills, Sex Differences, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Plumert, Jodie M.; Hund, Alycia M. – Child Development, 2001
Two experiments investigated the role of spatial prototypes in estimates of location. Found that adults and children ages 7 to 11 years overestimated distances between target locations in different regions and that none displaced objects toward the region centers. Even with boundaries removed during testing, adults and children overestimated…
Descriptors: Adults, Bias, Children, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Edward S.; Meade, Ann C. – Child Development, 1987
Data from a battery of spatial tests taken by children six to 18 years old indicate that a male advantage in spatial performance appears reliably by age 10, and that the magnitude of the advantage remains constant through age 18. (PCB)
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Females, Language Aptitude
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Linn, Marcia C.; Petersen, Anne C. – Child Development, 1985
Results suggest that (1) sex differences are found for some types of spatial ability but not others; (2) large sex differences are found only on measures of mental rotation; (3) smaller sex differences are found on measures of spatial perception; and (4) when sex differences are found, they can be detected across the life span. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Academic Ability, Adolescents, Adults, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vasta, Ross; Green, Pamela J. – Child Development, 1982
When reference cues are added to a pattern copying task, males' performance improves, but females' remains the same. This superior cue utilization may partially explain differences in spatial abilities. The present research attempts to determine the optimum locus for facilitation of copying by reference cues. (RH)
Descriptors: Children, Cues, Nature Nurture Controversy, Performance Factors
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rosser, Rosemary A. – Child Development, 1983
A total of 120 children between four to eight years of age were administered four sets of visual perspective-taking tasks. Results supported the hypothesis that children's task competence would be a fraction of the number and type of spatial relationships embedded in the stimulus displays. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Processes, Performance Factors
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2