NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)12
Since 2006 (last 20 years)37
Source
Journal of Experimental…41
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 41 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Majerus, Steve; Oberauer, Klaus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
The processing of ordinally organized information is a characteristic of both serial-order working memory and numerical cognition. Serial positions of items presented within a list follow an ordinal organization when stored in working memory, whereas numbers are based on an ordinal structure stored in long-term memory. We tested the hypothesis…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Serial Ordering, Numeracy, Numbers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lupker, Stephen J.; Nakayama, Mariko; Yoshihara, Masahiro – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
The present experiment provides an investigation of a promising new tool, the masked priming same-different task, for investigating the orthographic coding process. Orthographic coding is the process of establishing a mental representation of the letters and letter order in the word being read which is then used by readers to access higher-level…
Descriptors: Japanese, Phonology, Orthographic Symbols, Priming
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lehmer, Eva-Maria; Bäuml, Karl-Heinz T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
The results of four experiments are reported, in which we examined how the effects of part-list cuing--the presentation of a random selection of studied items as retrieval cues at test--on recall of the remaining target items depend on encoding and access to study context at test. Encoding was varied by inducing high and low degrees of interitem…
Descriptors: Cues, Recall (Psychology), Foreign Countries, College Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Salvaggio, Samuel; Masson, Nicolas; Andres, Michael – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
Behavioral studies have reported interactions between number processing and spatial attention, suggesting that number processing involves shifting attention along a mental continuum on which numbers are represented in ascending order. However, direct evidence for attention shifts remains scarce, the respective contribution of the horizontal and…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Spatial Ability, Coding, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yeo, Amelia; Alibali, Martha W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Past research suggests that speakers gesture more when motor simulations are more strongly activated. We investigate whether simulations of a perceptual nature also influence gesture production. Participants viewed animations of a spider moving with a manner of motion that was either highly salient (n = 29) or less salient (n = 31) and then…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Nonverbal Communication, Simulation, Animation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Dessel, Pieter; Eder, Andreas B.; Hughes, Sean – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Over the past decade an increasing number of studies across a range of domains have shown that the repeated performance of approach and avoidance (AA) actions in response to a stimulus leads to changes in the evaluation of that stimulus. The dominant (motivational-systems) account in this area claims that these effects are caused by a rewiring of…
Descriptors: Social Psychology, Motivation, Behavior, Training
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perea, Manuel; Jiménez, María; Gomez, Pablo – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
In the quest to unveil the nature of the orthographic code, a useful strategy is to examine the transposed-letter effect (e.g., JUGDE is more confusable with its base word, JUDGE, than the replacement-letter nonword JUPTE). A leading explanation of this phenomenon, which is in line with models of visual attention, is that there is perceptual…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Alphabets, Coding, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Robin, Jessica; Wynn, Jordana; Moscovitch, Morris – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Events always unfold in a spatial context, leading to the claim that it serves as a scaffold for encoding and retrieving episodic memories. The ubiquitous co-occurrence of spatial context with events may induce participants to generate a spatial context when hearing scenarios of events in which it is absent. Spatial context should also serve as an…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Recall (Psychology), Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Knutsen, Dominique; Le Bigot, Ludovic – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
As speakers interact, they add references to their common ground, which they can then reuse to facilitate listener comprehension. However, all references are not equally likely to be reused. The purpose of this study was to shed light on how the speakers' conceptualizations of the referents under discussion affect reuse (along with a generation…
Descriptors: Dialogs (Language), Interpersonal Communication, Memory, Perspective Taking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chu, Mingyuan; Kita, Sotaro – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
People spontaneously gesture when they speak (co-speech gestures) and when they solve problems silently (co-thought gestures). In this study, we first explored the relationship between these 2 types of gestures and found that individuals who produced co-thought gestures more frequently also produced co-speech gestures more frequently (Experiments…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Speech Communication, Correlation, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hilliard, Caitlin; Cook, Susan Wagner – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Communication is shaped both by what we are trying to say and by whom we are saying it to. We examined whether and how shared information influences the gestures speakers produce along with their speech. Unlike prior work examining effects of common ground on speech and gesture, we examined a situation in which some speakers have the same amount…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Experimental Psychology, Listening, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Giesen, Carina; Rothermund, Klaus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Stimulus-response (S-R) episodes are formed whenever a response is executed in close temporal proximity to a stimulus. Subsequent stimulus repetition will retrieve the episode from memory, reactivating the previous response. Whereas many research findings attest to the flexibility of representing stimulus features, only little is known about the…
Descriptors: Semantics, Motor Reactions, Task Analysis, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gambi, Chiara; Van de Cavey, Joris; Pickering, Martin J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
In 4 experiments we showed that picture naming latencies are affected by beliefs about the task concurrently performed by another speaker. Participants took longer to name pictures when they believed that their partner concurrently named pictures than when they believed their partner was silent (Experiments 1 and 4) or concurrently categorized the…
Descriptors: Interference (Learning), Barriers, Pictorial Stimuli, Naming
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Matthews, William J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
This article concerns the effect of context on people's judgments about sequences of chance outcomes. In Experiment 1, participants judged whether sequences were produced by random, mechanical processes (such as a roulette wheel) or skilled human action (such as basketball shots). Sequences with lower alternation rates were judged more likely to…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Probability, Prediction, Context Effect
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Slevc, L. Robert; Ferreira, Victor S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Natural language contains disfluencies and errors. Do listeners simply discard information that was clearly produced in error, or can erroneous material persist to affect subsequent processing? Two experiments explored this question using a structural priming paradigm. Speakers described dative-eliciting pictures after hearing prime sentences that…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Error Patterns, Priming, Syntax
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3