Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 27 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Ability | 44 |
Cognitive Development | 19 |
Task Analysis | 12 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Preschool Children | 9 |
Young Children | 9 |
Thinking Skills | 8 |
Child Development | 7 |
Children | 7 |
Beliefs | 6 |
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 44 |
Author
Bjorklund, David F. | 2 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 2 |
Alexander, Joyce M. | 1 |
Amsel, Eric | 1 |
Anastasiou, Maria | 1 |
Aureli, Tiziana | 1 |
Bailey, Drew H. | 1 |
Beck, Erinn F. | 1 |
Behne, Tanya | 1 |
Bell, Martha Ann | 1 |
Bellana, Buddhika | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 44 |
Reports - Research | 32 |
Reports - Evaluative | 9 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 5 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
Grade 1 | 2 |
Grade 2 | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Greenberg, Anastasia; Bellana, Buddhika; Bialystok, Ellen – Cognitive Development, 2013
Monolingual and bilingual 8-year-olds performed a computerized spatial perspective-taking task. Children were asked to decide how an observer saw a four-block array from one of three different positions (90 degrees, 180 degrees, and 270 degrees counter-clockwise from the child's position) by selecting one of four responses--the correct response,…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Monolingualism, Children, Executive Function
Frick, Andrea; Newcombe, Nora S. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Spatial scaling is an integral aspect of many spatial tasks that involve symbol-to-referent correspondences (e.g., map reading, drawing). In this study, we asked 3-6-year-olds and adults to locate objects in a two-dimensional spatial layout using information from a second spatial representation (map). We examined how scaling factor and reference…
Descriptors: Scaling, Spatial Ability, Toddlers, Young Children
Weisberg, Deena Skolnick; Sobel, David M. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Can young children discriminate impossible events, which cannot happen in reality, from improbable events, which are unfamiliar but could possibly happen in reality? When asked explicitly to categorize these types of events, 4-year-olds (N = 54) tended to report that improbable events were impossible, consistent with prior results (Shtulman &…
Descriptors: Young Children, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Classification
Mercier, Hugo – Cognitive Development, 2011
The argumentative theory of reasoning (Mercier & Sperber, in press) claims that reasoning evolved for argumentation: to find and evaluate arguments in dialogic contexts. The theory has drawn most of its supportive evidence from work with adults, leaving open the possibility that argumentive features of reasoning are in fact entirely learned.…
Descriptors: Persuasive Discourse, Thinking Skills, Children, Cognitive Ability
Brandone, Amanda C.; Gelman, Susan A. – Cognitive Development, 2013
The goal of the present study was to explore domain differences in young children's expectations about the structure of animal and artifact categories. We examined 5-year-olds' and adults' use of category-referring generic noun phrases (e.g., "Birds fly") about novel animals and artifacts. The same stimuli served as both animals and artifacts;…
Descriptors: Animals, Language Usage, Language Acquisition, Cues
Does High-Level Intellectual Performance Depend on Practice Alone? Debunking the Polgar Sisters Case
Howard, Robert W. – Cognitive Development, 2011
The famous Polgar sisters started chess very young, undertook extensive study, and two became grandmasters. This case often is cited as decisive evidence that practice alone is key in development of expertise, that innate talent is unimportant or non-existent, and that almost anyone can become a grandmaster. But on close examination these claims…
Descriptors: Expertise, Cognitive Ability, Performance Factors, Siblings
Jansen, P.; Schmelter, A.; Quaiser-Pohl, C.; Neuburger, S.; Heil, M. – Cognitive Development, 2013
In contrast to the well documented male advantage in psychometric mental rotation tests, gender differences in chronometric experimental designs are still under dispute. Therefore, a systematic investigation of gender differences in mental rotation performance in primary-school children is presented in this paper. A chronometric mental rotation…
Descriptors: Animals, Visual Stimuli, Pictorial Stimuli, Psychometrics
Gardiner, Amy K.; Bjorklund, David F.; Greif, Marissa L.; Gray, Sarah K. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Children's acquisition of tool use abilities is an important part of development but is not yet well understood. This study compares two modes of tool-use learning, observation and individual haptic experience. Two- and 3-year-olds had haptic experience with tools, observed tool use by others, had both haptic and observational experience, or no…
Descriptors: Observation, Task Analysis, Difficulty Level, Cognitive Ability
McNeil, Nicole M.; Fuhs, Mary Wagner; Keultjes, M. Claire; Gibson, Matthew H. – Cognitive Development, 2011
Recent studies suggest that 5-year-olds can add and compare large numerical quantities through approximate representations of number. However, the nature of this understanding and its susceptibility to environmental influences remain unclear. We examined whether children's early competence depends on the canonical problem format (i.e., arithmetic…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Status, Environmental Influences, Arithmetic, Preschool Children
Wang, Fei; Zhu, Liqi; Shi, Kan – Cognitive Development, 2011
We investigated how 3-7-year-olds weigh and coordinate information about specific mental states with social norms in the domain of contextually conventional rules. With increasing age, participants increasingly took into account an actor's mental state to predict that actor's behavior. In a criticism judgment task, 7-year-olds could assign…
Descriptors: Criticism, Norms, Cognitive Development, Behavior Standards
Friedman, William J. – Cognitive Development, 2011
Recent research on children's thinking about the future has taken multiple directions, many of which are illustrated in the contributions to this special issue. In this commentary the topic is discussed in the context of research on children's understanding of time, and some of the adaptive challenges of thinking about the future are considered.…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Research, Children, Thinking Skills
Lemaire, Patrick; Lecacheur, Mireille – Cognitive Development, 2011
Third, fifth, and seventh graders selected the best strategy (rounding up or rounding down) for estimating answers to two-digit addition problems. Executive function measures were collected for each individual. Data showed that (a) children's skill at both strategy selection and execution improved with age and (b) increased efficiency in executive…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Grade 5, Grade 7, Age Differences
Aureli, Tiziana; Perucchini, Paola; Genco, Maria – Cognitive Development, 2009
Two tasks were administered to 40 children aged from 16 to 20 months (mean age = 18;1), to evaluate children's understanding of declarative and informative intention [Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. "Developmental Science", 8, 492-499;…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Nonverbal Communication, Intention, Cognitive Ability
Geary, David C.; Bailey, Drew H.; Littlefield, Andrew; Wood, Phillip; Hoard, Mary K.; Nugent, Lara – Cognitive Development, 2009
Kindergarten to third grade mathematics achievement scores from a prospective study of mathematical development (n = 306) were subjected to latent growth trajectory analyses. The four corresponding classes included children with mathematical learning disability (MLD, 6% of sample), and low (LA, 50%), typically (TA, 39%) and high (HA, 5%) achieving…
Descriptors: Learning Disabilities, Mathematics Achievement, Intelligence Quotient, At Risk Students
Grafenhain, Maria; Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda; Tomasello, Michael – Cognitive Development, 2009
We investigated whether infants comprehend others' nonverbal communicative intentions directed to a third person, in an "overhearing" context. An experimenter addressed an assistant and indicated a hidden toy's location by either gazing ostensively or pointing to the location for her. In a matched control condition, the experimenter performed…
Descriptors: Cues, Interpersonal Communication, Infants, Comprehension