NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 82 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berthiaume, Vincent G.; Shultz, Thomas R.; Onishi, Kristine H. – Cognition, 2013
How do children come to understand that others have mental representations, e.g., of an object's location? Preschoolers go through two transitions on verbal false-belief tasks, in which they have to predict where an agent will search for an object that was moved in her absence. First, while three-and-a-half-year-olds usually fail at approach…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Cognitive Development, Constructivism (Learning), Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schmidt, Marco F. H.; Rakoczy, Hannes; Tomasello, Michael – Cognition, 2012
To become cooperative members of their cultural groups, developing children must follow their group's social norms. But young children are not just blind norm followers, they are also active norm enforcers, for example, protesting and correcting when someone plays a conventional game the "wrong" way. In two studies, we asked whether young children…
Descriptors: Young Children, Norms, Child Development, Games
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cimpian, Andrei; Scott, Rose M. – Cognition, 2012
The ability to acquire and store generic information (that is, information about entire categories) is at the core of human cognition. Remarkably, even young children place special value on generic information, often inferring that it holds important insights about the world. Here, we tested whether children's assumptions about the nature of…
Descriptors: Social Cognition, Language Acquisition, Experiments, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lee, Sang Ah; Sovrano, Valeria A.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2012
Geometry is one of the highest achievements of our species, but its foundations are obscure. Consistent with longstanding suggestions that geometrical knowledge is rooted in processes guiding navigation, the present study examines potential sources of geometrical knowledge in the navigation processes by which young children establish their sense…
Descriptors: Young Children, Geometric Concepts, Geometry, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rottman, Joshua; Kelemen, Deborah – Cognition, 2012
The traditional cognitive developmental perspective on moral acquisition posits that children actively construct moral beliefs by assessing the negative impacts of antisocial behaviors. This account is not easily applied to actions that are considered immoral despite lacking consequences for others' welfare. We studied the moralization of…
Descriptors: Norms, Moral Values, Moral Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rowland, Caroline F.; Chang, Franklin; Ambridge, Ben; Pine, Julian M.; Lieven, Elena V. M. – Cognition, 2012
Structural priming paradigms have been influential in shaping theories of adult sentence processing and theories of syntactic development. However, until recently there have been few attempts to provide an integrated account that explains both adult and developmental data. The aim of the present paper was to begin the process of integration by…
Descriptors: Priming, Evidence, Sentences, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De Bruin, L. C.; Newen, A. – Cognition, 2012
The elicited-response false belief task has traditionally been considered as reliably indicating that children acquire an understanding of false belief around 4 years of age. However, recent investigations using spontaneous-response tasks suggest that false belief understanding emerges much earlier. This leads to a developmental paradox: if young…
Descriptors: Investigations, Preschool Children, Infants, Organizations (Groups)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roder, Brigitte; Pagel, Birthe; Heed, Tobias – Cognition, 2013
The integrated use of spatial and temporal information seems to support the separation of two sensory streams. The present study tested whether this facilitation depends on the encoding of sensory stimuli in externally anchored spatial coordinate systems. Fifty-nine children between 5 and 12 years as well as 12 young adults performed a crossmodal…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes, Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Luo, Yuyan – Cognition, 2011
As adults, we know that others' mental states, such as beliefs, guide their behavior and that these mental states can deviate from reality. Researchers have examined whether young children possess adult-like theory of mind by focusing on their understanding about others' false beliefs. The present research revealed that 10-month-old infants seemed…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Infants, Toys, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mahajan, Neha; Wynn, Karen – Cognition, 2012
A central feature of human psychology is our pervasive tendency to divide the social world into "us" and "them". We prefer to associate with those who are similar to us over those who are different, preferentially allocate resources to similar others, and hold more positive beliefs about similar others. Here we investigate the developmental…
Descriptors: Infants, Interpersonal Attraction, Values, Cultural Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berteletti, Ilaria; Lucangeli, Daniela; Zorzi, Marco – Cognition, 2012
The representation of numerical and non-numerical ordered sequences was investigated in children from preschool to grade 3. The child's conception of how sequence items map onto a spatial scale was tested using the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and new variants of the task designed to probe the representation of the alphabet…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Investigations, Preschool Education, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fisher, Anna V. – Cognition, 2011
Is processing of conceptual information as robust as processing of perceptual information early in development? Existing empirical evidence is insufficient to answer this question. To examine this issue, 3- to 5-year-old children were presented with a flexible categorization task, in which target items (e.g., an open red umbrella) shared category…
Descriptors: Test Items, Classification, Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marentette, Paula; Nicoladis, Elena – Cognition, 2011
This study explores a common assumption made in the cognitive development literature that children will treat gestures as labels for objects. Without doubt, researchers in these experiments intend to use gestures symbolically as labels. The present studies examine whether children interpret these gestures as labels. In Study 1 two-, three-, and…
Descriptors: Nouns, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schapiro, Anna C.; McClelland, James L. – Cognition, 2009
A connectionist model of the balance scale task is presented which exhibits developmental transitions between "Rule I" and "Rule II" behavior [Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. "Cognitive Psychology," 8, 481-520.] as well as the "catastrophe flags" seen in data from Jansen and van der Maas [Jansen, B. R. J., & van der…
Descriptors: Cognitive Psychology, Cognitive Development, Children, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Vasilyeva, Marina; Newcombe, Nora; Duffy, Sean – Cognition, 2008
The present research examines the ability of children as young as 4 years to use models in tasks that require scaling of distance along a single dimension. In Experiment 1, we found that tasks involving models are similar in difficulty to those involving maps that we studied earlier (Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Vasilyeva, M. (1999). Spatial…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Play, Scaling, Models
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6