NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Researchers4
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 38 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wanjiang Zhou; Pablo Saiz-González; Ronny Rodriguez Aragon; Kaitlyn Adams; Zan Gao – Quest, 2024
This systematic review synthesized current literature regarding the effect of physical activity (PA) interventions on brain structure (BS) and brain function (BF) in healthy children. This review followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews protocols and used the Rayyan web for data extraction. Eleven experimental studies were…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Intervention, Cognitive Structures, Brain
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brito, Natalie H.; Noble, Kimberly G. – Developmental Science, 2018
Family socioeconomic status (SES) is strongly associated with children's cognitive development, and past studies have reported socioeconomic disparities in both neurocognitive skills and brain structure across childhood. In other studies, bilingualism has been associated with cognitive advantages and differences in brain structure across the…
Descriptors: Family Income, Family Characteristics, Socioeconomic Status, Bilingualism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Slusser, Emily B.; Santiago, Rachel T.; Barth, Hilary C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2013
Mental representations of numerical magnitude are commonly thought to undergo discontinuous change over development in the form of a "representational shift." This idea stems from an apparent categorical shift from logarithmic to linear patterns of numerical estimation on tasks that involve translating between numerical magnitudes and…
Descriptors: Children, Computation, Numbers, Change
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dixon, James A.; Boncoddo, Rebecca – Cognitive Development, 2009
In an accompanying study, Alibali et al. [Alibali, M. W., Ockuly, K. M., Fischer, A. D. (2009) "Learning new problem-solving strategies leads to changes in problem representation." "Cognitive Development, 24," 89-101.] present an important experimental result: introducing a new strategy can affect conceptual aspects of children's problem…
Descriptors: Cognitive Structures, Problem Solving, Cognitive Development, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Astington, Janet Wilde – Human Development, 1998
Focuses on Nelson et al's use of the term "theory of mind" and its meaning; alternatives to theory of mind; theories of theory of mind; arguments against theory of mind; and language and theory of mind. (KB)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Metacognition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Overton, Willis F.; Muller, Ulrich – Human Development, 1998
Replies to commentaries by Mandler (1988) and Rochat and Striano (1988), focusing on Mandler's comments. Maintains that their disagreements are the result of deep meta-theoretical differences regarding a representational theory of mind rather than misrepresentations of fact. Discusses how their meta-theoretical differences result in several basic…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gelman, Rochel – Child Development, 2000
Maintains that there are core-specific and non-core-specific domains of knowledge, but that only the core-specific domains benefit from innate skeletal structures. Asserts that core skeletal domains are universally shared, even though their particular foci may vary. Emphasizes that individuals vary in terms of the noncore domains they acquire.…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cinan, Sevtap – Cognitive Development, 2006
This study examined developmental changes in concept formation, rule switching, and perseverative behaviors of children in the WCST by altering visual features of the test and using a new test score--the "zigzag" error score--which shows the number of shifts made between two incorrect concepts or rules. Instead of the original four 3-dimensional…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Scores, Cognitive Development, Persistence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lillard, Angeline – Human Development, 1998
Notes that Nelson, Plesa, and Henseler's (1998) article addresses the issues of where social cognitive knowledge comes from, what form it takes, and whether "theory of mind" is an appropriate description of the social cognitive enterprise. Argues that researchers ought to get beyond the "theory" issue, and focus on the sources…
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Molenaar, Peter C. M.; van der Maas, Han L. J. – Human Development, 1994
Comments on Lewis's ideas about reconciling stage and specificity in neo-Piagetian theory in this issue. Focuses on whether general stages, domain specificity, and individual diversity are compatible from a nonlinear, dynamic perspective. Suggests that, by using catastrophe theory, intra- and interindividual diversity and domain specificity can be…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Kathy E.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1992
Judgments of similarity among mammals, including humans, were elicited from 7 and 10 year olds and adults. Results partially supported a consensus model of shared cultural knowledge. Patterns of deviation from the model appeared between children and adults because of the emergence of a primate category after age 10. (BC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lewis, Marc D. – Human Development, 1994
To resolve tension between general stages and conceptual specificity in neo-Piagetian theory, R. Case introduced the idea of central conceptual structures. To resolve difficulties of separating developmental level and conceptual diversity, this article reconceptualizes central conceptual structures as self-organizing systems that stabilize in…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
de Ribaupierre, Anik – Human Development, 1994
Comments on Lewis's ideas about reconciling stage and specificity in neo-Piagetian theory in this issue. Summarizes R. Case's central conceptual structure and its relation to other neo-Piagetian theories. Notes similarities between Lewis and Piaget, suggesting that differences adhere to a limited number of general laws instead of being…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Burks, Virginia Salzer; Dodge, Kenneth A.; Price, Joseph M.; Laird, Robert D. – Developmental Psychology, 1999
Investigated the relationship between children's knowledge structures for peers and externalizing behavior problems. Evaluated initial aggression level in years 1, 6, and 9 of longitudinal study; evaluated social knowledge structures in year 6. Found that knowledge structures were related to children's concurrent externalizing behavior and…
Descriptors: Aggression, Behavior Problems, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Thornton, Stephanie – Child Development, 1999
Proposes that conceptual change is constrained by the child's conceptual structures and the structures inherent in problem-solving tasks. Uses a microgenetic case study and group data to examine how interaction between strategies children bring to a task and the detailed task structure redirect children's attention and create the possibility of…
Descriptors: Attention, Case Studies, Children, Cognitive Development
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3