Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 35 |
Perceptual Development | 35 |
Visual Stimuli | 35 |
Visual Perception | 17 |
Infants | 15 |
Age Differences | 13 |
Cognitive Processes | 9 |
Research | 7 |
Young Children | 6 |
Attention | 5 |
Child Development | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 17 |
Reports - Research | 17 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Books | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lyons, Kristen E.; Ghetti, Simona – Child Development, 2013
Although some evidence indicates that even very young children engage in rudimentary forms of strategic behavior, the underlying mechanisms remain largely unknown. This study tested the hypothesis that uncertainty monitoring underlies such behaviors. Three-, four-, and five-year-old children ("N" = 88) completed a perceptual…
Descriptors: Child Development, Behavior Problems, Hypothesis Testing, Individual Differences
Bartolucci, Marco; Smith, Andrew T. – Neuropsychologia, 2011
Practicing a visual task commonly results in improved performance. Often the improvement does not transfer well to a new retinal location, suggesting that it is mediated by changes occurring in early visual cortex, and indeed neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies both demonstrate that perceptual learning is associated with altered activity…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Perceptual Development, Attention
Aslin, Richard N. – Developmental Science, 2007
The most common behavioral technique used to study infant perception, cognition, language, and social development is some variant of looking time. Since its inception as a reliable method in the late 1950s, a tremendous increase in knowledge about infant competencies has been gained by inferences made from measures of looking time. Here we examine…
Descriptors: Infants, Inferences, Perception, Cognitive Development

Ruff, Holly A.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1976
Recognition by infants, 13 and 22 weeks old, was tested by pairing novel stimuli with the familiarization stimulus at different points in an experimental session. Younger subjects showed no recognition of either two- or three- dimensional stimuli. Older subjects demonstrated more recognition in the three-dimensional condition. (MS)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Infants, Perceptual Development

Milewski, Allan E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976
Human infants' discrimination of changes in internal and external elements of compound visual patterns was investigated in four experiments employing a familiarization-novelty paradigm in which visual reinforcing patterns were presented contingent upon rate of high-amplitude nonnutritive sucking. (Author/JH)
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Development, Infants, Perceptual Development

Rose, Susan A – Child Development, 1988
Investigated infants' integration of visual information across space and time. In four experiments, infants aged 12 months and 6 months viewed objects after watching light trace similar and dissimilar shapes. Infants looked longer at novel shapes, although six-month-olds did not recognize figures taking more than 10 seconds to trace. One-year-old…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infants, Perceptual Development, Psychological Studies

Rose, Susan Ann – Child Development, 1977
Two studies: (1) assessed the infant's ability to perceive differences between two-dimensional and three-dimensional stimuli; and (2) tested the infants' ability to transfer responses across dimensions. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Eye Fixations, Infants, Perceptual Development

Greenberg, David J.; And Others – Child Development, 1973
To demonstrate a relationship between rate of habituation and complexity levels, 11-week-old infants (N=51) were each given a rate-of-habituation and complexity-level test. Rapid habituators looked longer at complex patterns. Irregular habituators responded randomly to tests or resembled slow habituators in terms of complexity preferred. (ST)
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Development, Difficulty Level, Infants

Lefton, Lester A.; Fisher, Dennis F. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976
Five developmental experiments examine the role of context in visual search. (Author/JH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, College Students, Grade 3, Grade 5

McKenzie, B. E.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Two experiments found that (1) by age 8 months infants perceived that leaning extends their effective reaching space to grasp objects; (2) by 10 months they perceived the effective limits of leaning and reaching; and (3) by 12 months they began to perceive how this space may be extended by a mechanical aid. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Infants, Perceptual Development

Corballis, Michael C.; Zalik, Marsha C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977
This experiment was designed to determine whether children's inability to discriminate mirror-image oblique lines is a function of encoding in memory the left-right orientation or the degree of slope of an oblique line. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Memory, Perceptual Development

Smith, Linda B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1981
The hypothesis that overall-similarity relations structure both adults' and children's classifications of heterogeneous objects (objects that differ in a variety of ways) was supported in two experiments. When objects varied simultaneously on many dimensions, adults and children constructed classifications that maximized within-category similarity…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Dimensional Preference

Schlottmann, Anne; Allen, Deborah; Linderoth, Carina; Hesketh, Sarah – Child Development, 2002
Three experiments examined development of perceptual causality in 3- to 9-year-olds. Findings indicated that participants of all ages assigned contact events (A moves toward B, which moves upon contact) to the physical domain and non-contact events (B moves before contact) to the psychological domain. Participants chose causality more often for…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Causal Models, Children, Cognitive Development

Fantz, Robert L.; Miranda, Simon B. – Child Development, 1973
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Development, Dimensional Preference, Downs Syndrome

Arterberry, Martha E.; Bornstein, Marc H. – Cognition, 2002
Five experiments used a categorization habituation-of-looking paradigm to investigate infants' categorization of animals and vehicles based on static versus dynamic attributes of stimuli (color images versus dynamic point-light displays). Findings showed that 6-month-olds categorize animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic information, and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis