NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 15 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Amanda Saksida; Alan Langus – Child Development, 2024
The account that word learning starts in earnest during the second year of life, when infants have mastered the disambiguation skills, has recently been challenged by evidence that infants during the first year already know many common words. The preliminary ability to rapidly map and disambiguate linguistic labels was tested in Italian-speaking…
Descriptors: Naming, Infants, Cognitive Mapping, Vocabulary Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Burles, Ford; Liu, Irene; Hart, Chelsie; Murias, Kara; Graham, Susan A.; Iaria, Giuseppe – Child Development, 2020
Although much is known about adults' ability to orient by means of cognitive maps (mental representations of the environment), it is less clear when this important ability emerges in development. In the present study, 97 seven- to 10-year-olds and 26 adults played a video game designed to investigate the ability to orient using cognitive maps. The…
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Spatial Ability, Children, Navigation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lucca, Kelsey; Wilbourn, Makeba Parramore – Child Development, 2018
Infants' pointing gestures are a critical predictor of early vocabulary size. However, it remains unknown precisely how pointing relates to word learning. The current study addressed this question in a sample of 108 infants, testing one mechanism by which infants' pointing may influence their learning. In Study 1, 18-month-olds, but not…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Child Development, Predictor Variables
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Havy, Mélanie; Foroud, Afra; Fais, Laurel; Werker, Janet F. – Child Development, 2017
Visual information influences speech perception in both infants and adults. It is still unknown whether lexical representations are multisensory. To address this question, we exposed 18-month-old infants (n = 32) and adults (n = 32) to new word-object pairings: Participants either heard the acoustic form of the words or saw the talking face in…
Descriptors: Infants, Vocabulary Development, Adults, Speech
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hay, Jessica F.; Graf Estes, Katharine; Wang, Tianlin; Saffran, Jenny R. – Child Development, 2015
Infants must develop both flexibility and constraint in their interpretation of acceptable word forms. The current experiments examined the development of infants' lexical interpretation of non-native variations in pitch contour. Fourteen-, 17-, and 19-month-olds (Experiments 1 and 2, N = 72) heard labels for two novel objects; labels…
Descriptors: Infants, Intonation, Auditory Perception, Suprasegmentals
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gentner, Dedre; Anggoro, Florencia K.; Klibanoff, Raquel S. – Child Development, 2011
Learning relational categories--whose membership is defined not by intrinsic properties but by extrinsic relations with other entities--poses a challenge to young children. The current work showed 3-, 4- to 5-, and 6-year-olds pairs of cards exemplifying familiar relations (e.g., a nest and a bird exemplifying "home for") and then tested whether…
Descriptors: Young Children, Learning, Classification, Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Imai, Mutsumi; Li, Lianjing; Haryu, Etsuko; Okada, Hiroyuki; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Shigematsu, Jun – Child Development, 2008
When can children speaking Japanese, English, or Chinese map and extend novel nouns and verbs? Across 6 studies, 3- and 5-year-old children in all 3 languages map and extend novel nouns more readily than novel verbs. This finding prevails even in languages like Chinese and Japanese that are assumed to be verb-friendly languages (e.g., T. Tardif,…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Grammar, Japanese
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lipton, Jennifer S.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Child Development, 2005
Five-year-old children categorized as skilled versus unskilled counters were given verbal estimation and number word comprehension tasks with numerosities 20-120. Skilled counters showed a linear relation between number words and nonsymbolic numerosities. Unskilled counters showed the same linear relation for smaller numbers to which they could…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Numbers, Cognitive Mapping, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Baldwin, Dare A. – Child Development, 1991
Labels for toys were taught to 64 infants. In follow-in labeling, the experimenter labeled a toy at which infants were looking; in discrepant labeling, one at which they were not looking. Results revealed that infants learned follow-in labels and made no mapping errors after discrepant labeling. (BC)
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Mapping, Cues, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ellis, Wendy E.; Zarbatany, Lynne – Child Development, 2007
Group status was examined as a moderator of peer group socialization of deviant, aggressive, and prosocial behavior. In the fall and 3 months later, preadolescents and early adolescents provided self-reported scores for deviant behavior and group membership, and peer nominations for overt and relational aggression, prosocial behavior, and social…
Descriptors: Peer Groups, Socialization, Prosocial Behavior, Group Membership
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Imai, Mutsumi; Haryu, Etsuko; Okada, Hiroyuki – Child Development, 2005
The present research examined how 3- and 5-year-old Japanese children map novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events and generalize them to new instances. Studies 1 to 3 demonstrated that although both 3- and 5-year-olds were able to map novel nouns onto novel objects, only 5-year-olds could generalize verbs solely on the basis of the…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Anooshian, Linda J.; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Results of two studies suggested that acquisition of route mapping during the preschool years provides a means of organizing spatial information and internal representations necessary for successful problem solving in general. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Mapping, Early Experience
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lederberg, Amy R.; Prezbindowski, Amy K.; Spencer, Patricia E. – Child Development, 2000
Assessed word-learning skills of 19 deaf/hard-of-hearing preschoolers either with novel mapping strategy to learn new words, or after minimal exposure when reference was explicitly established. Found that 11 children were able to learn words in both contexts, 5 only in the second, and 2 in neither. The latter 7 children eventually were able to…
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Context Effect, Deafness, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pahl, Kerstin; Way, Niobe – Child Development, 2006
The current study modeled developmental trajectories of ethnic identity exploration and affirmation and belonging from middle to late adolescence (ages 15-18) and examined how these trajectories varied according to ethnicity, gender, immigrant status, and perceived level of discrimination. The sample consisted of 135 urban low-income Black and…
Descriptors: Ethnicity, African Americans, Hispanic Americans, Late Adolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
DeLoache, Judy S.; And Others – Child Development, 1991
Tested understanding of correspondence on the part of 2.5- to 3.5-year olds who watched a toy hidden in a model and tried to find an analogous toy in a room. Retrieval scores increased with increasing model-room similarity; were higher for older than younger children; and were affected by object and size similarity. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Mapping, Individual Development