NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 25 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bion, Ricardo A. H.; Borovsky, Arielle; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 2013
When hearing a novel name, children tend to select a novel object rather than a familiar one, a bias known as disambiguation. Using online processing measures with 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds, we investigate how the development of this bias relates to word learning. Children's proportion of looking time to a novel object after hearing a novel name…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Novels, Vocabulary Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Monaghan, Padraic; Mattock, Karen – Cognition, 2012
Learning word-referent mappings is complex because the word and its referent tend to co-occur with multiple other words and potential referents. Such complexity has led to proposals for a host of constraints on learning, though how these constraints may interact has not yet been investigated in detail. In this paper, we investigated interactions…
Descriptors: Phonology, Form Classes (Languages), Cognitive Mapping, Investigations
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Takac, Martin; Benuskova, Lubica; Knott, Alistair – Cognition, 2012
In this article we present a neural network model of sentence generation. The network has both technical and conceptual innovations. Its main technical novelty is in its semantic representations: the messages which form the input to the network are structured as sequences, so that message elements are delivered to the network one at a time. Rather…
Descriptors: Semantics, Language Acquisition, Sentences, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gebuis, Titia; Gevers, Wim – Cognition, 2011
de Hevia and Spelke (de Hevia and Spelke (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children, "Cognition, 110", 198-207) investigated the mapping of number onto space. To this end, they introduced a non-symbolic flanker task. Here subjects have to bisect a line that is flanked by a 2-dot and a 9-dot array. Similar to the…
Descriptors: Cues, Cognitive Processes, Investigations, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berteletti, Ilaria; Lucangeli, Daniela; Zorzi, Marco – Cognition, 2012
The representation of numerical and non-numerical ordered sequences was investigated in children from preschool to grade 3. The child's conception of how sequence items map onto a spatial scale was tested using the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and new variants of the task designed to probe the representation of the alphabet…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Investigations, Preschool Education, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Saji, Noburo; Imai, Mutsumi; Saalbach, Henrik; Zhang, Yuping; Shu, Hua; Okada, Hiroyuki – Cognition, 2011
This paper explores the process through which children sort out the relations among verbs belonging to the same semantic domain. Using a set of Chinese verbs denoting a range of action events that are labeled by carrying or holding in English as a test case, we looked at how Chinese-speaking 3-, 5-, and 7-year-olds and adults apply 13 different…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Vocabulary Development, Chinese
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sumner, Meghan – Cognition, 2011
Phonetic variation has been considered a barrier that listeners must overcome in speech perception, but has been proved beneficial in category learning. In this paper, I show that listeners use within-speaker variation to accommodate gross categorical variation. Within the perceptual learning paradigm, listeners are exposed to p-initial words in…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Auditory Perception, Native Speakers, Speech Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ulrich, Rolf; Maienborn, Claudia – Cognition, 2010
The metaphoric mapping theory suggests that abstract concepts, like time, are represented in terms of concrete dimensions such as space. This theory receives support from several lines of research ranging from psychophysics to linguistics and cultural studies; especially strong support comes from recent response time studies. These studies have…
Descriptors: Sentences, Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes, Coding
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Oppenheimer, Daniel M.; LeBoeuf, Robyn A.; Brewer, Noel T. – Cognition, 2008
Research has shown that judgments tend to assimilate to irrelevant "anchors." We extend anchoring effects to show that anchors can even operate across modalities by, apparently, priming a general sense of magnitude that is not moored to any unit or scale. An initial study showed that participants drawing long "anchor" lines made higher numerical…
Descriptors: Learning Modalities, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lyons, Ian M.; Beilock, Sian L. – Cognition, 2009
In two different contexts, we examined the hypothesis that individual differences in working memory (WM) capacity are related to the tendency to infer complex, ordinal relationships "between" numerical symbols. In Experiment 1, we assessed whether this tendency arises in a learning context that involves mapping novel symbols to quantities by…
Descriptors: Numeracy, Short Term Memory, Individual Differences, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Altmann, Gerry T. M.; Kamide, Yuki – Cognition, 2009
Two experiments explored the mapping between language and mental representations of visual scenes. In both experiments, participants viewed, for example, a scene depicting a woman, a wine glass and bottle on the floor, an empty table, and various other objects. In Experiment 1, participants concurrently heard either "The woman will put the glass…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Eye Movements, Oral Language, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Eitan, Zohar; Timmers, Renee – Cognition, 2010
Though auditory pitch is customarily mapped in Western cultures onto spatial verticality (high-low), both anthropological reports and cognitive studies suggest that pitch may be mapped onto a wide variety of other domains. We collected a total number of 35 pitch mappings and investigated in four experiments how these mappings are used and…
Descriptors: Music, Figurative Language, Cognitive Processes, Intonation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vouloumanos, Athena – Cognition, 2008
A language learner trying to acquire a new word must often sift through many potential relations between particular words and their possible meanings. In principle, statistical information about the distribution of those mappings could serve as one important source of data, but little is known about whether learners can in fact track multiple…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Word Recognition, Cognitive Mapping, Bayesian Statistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
You, Wenping; Chen, Baoguo; Dunlap, Susan – Cognition, 2009
Frequency trajectory is a better measure to investigate age-limited learning effects than age of acquisition (AoA) ratings (Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). "Age of acquisition effects in word reading and other tasks." "Journal of Memory and Language, 47"(1), 1-29). The current study uses frequency trajectory as a variable to investigate…
Descriptors: Semantics, Written Language, Character Recognition, Chinese
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Preissler, Melissa Allen; Bloom, Paul – Cognition, 2008
Adults appreciate that an abstract visual representation can be understood through inferring the artist's intention. Many investigators have argued that this capacity is a late-emerging developmental accomplishment, a claim supported by findings that preschool children ignore explicit statements about intent when naming pictures. Using a…
Descriptors: Artists, Intention, Toddlers, Freehand Drawing
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2