Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Mapping | 13 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Preschool Children | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Nouns | 3 |
Primary Education | 3 |
Spatial Ability | 3 |
Young Children | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 13 |
Author
Campbell, Jennifer | 1 |
Carre, Rene | 1 |
Chad Spiegel | 1 |
Chen, Zhe | 1 |
Chicos, Kelly A. | 1 |
Curtin, Suzanne | 1 |
Daehler, Marvin W. | 1 |
Deak, Gedeon O. | 1 |
Ebersbach, Mirjam | 1 |
Herold, Debora S. | 1 |
Hufnagle, Dan | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 13 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Location
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Deak, Gedeon O.; Toney, Alexis J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
To test general and specific processes of symbol learning, 4- and 5-year-old children learned three kinds of abstract associates for novel objects: words, facts, and pictograms. To test fast mapping (i.e., one-trial learning) and subsequent learning, comprehension was tested after each of four exposures. Production was also tested, as was…
Descriptors: Young Children, Cognitive Mapping, Generalization, Bias
Curtin, Suzanne; Campbell, Jennifer; Hufnagle, Dan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We investigated the effect of lexical stress on 16-month-olds' ability to form associations between labels and paths of motion. Disyllabic English nouns tend to have a strong-weak (trochaic) stress pattern, and verbs tend to have a weak-strong (iambic) pattern. We explored whether infants would use word stress information to guide word-action…
Descriptors: Suprasegmentals, Nouns, Infants, Organizations (Groups)
Herold, Debora S.; Nygaard, Lynne C.; Chicos, Kelly A.; Namy, Laura L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
This study examined whether children use prosodic correlates to word meaning when interpreting novel words. For example, do children infer that a word spoken in a deep, slow, loud voice refers to something larger than a word spoken in a high, fast, quiet voice? Participants were 4- and 5-year-olds who viewed picture pairs that varied along a…
Descriptors: Cues, Semantics, Vocabulary Development, Intonation
Chad Spiegel; Justin Halberda – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Learning a new word consists of two primary tasks that have often been conflated into a single process: "referent selection", in which a child must determine the correct referent of a novel label, and "referent retention", which is the ability to store this newly formed label-object mapping in memory for later use. In addition,…
Descriptors: Nouns, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Language Acquisition, Task Analysis
Ebersbach, Mirjam; Resing, Wilma C. M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
Two experiments using the "projection of shadows" paradigm investigated multidimensional reasoning, implicit and explicit knowledge, and the nonlinearity concept in 5-, 9-, and 13-year-olds and adults. Participants estimated the resulting shadow lengths of differently sized objects, placed at varying distances from a light source. Experiment 1…
Descriptors: Knowledge Level, Children, Early Adolescents, Age Differences

Wilkinson, Krista M.; McIlvane, William J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1997
Examined a blank comparison method for evaluating emergent symbol mapping and learning of new word: picture matching relations by 3- to 5-year olds. Found that the method had considerable promise for advancing theoretical analyses of emergent mapping in behavior analytic and developmental language research. (KB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Preschool Children

Morrongiello, Barbara A.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1995
Studied spatial knowledge in fully blind versus fully sighted four- to nine-year olds. Found that blind children performed as well as sighted on all tasks but one. (ETB)
Descriptors: Blindness, Children, Cognitive Mapping, Encoding (Psychology)

Chen, Zhe; Daehler, Marvin W. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1992
Kindergartners and second graders heard stories containing an intention to solve a problem and a successful outcome or stories that lacked these components. Second graders showed evidence of transfer of knowledge for stories containing an intentional component. (BC)
Descriptors: Analogy, Cognitive Mapping, Elementary School Students, Intention

Merriman, William E.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1993
Two experiments explored the tendency of preschoolers to map novel nouns and verbs onto unfamiliar rather than familiar objects or actions. This disambiguation effect has been interpreted as evidence that youngsters expect object or action labels to be mutually exclusive. The effect was stronger for object than for action words. (MDM)
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Cognitive Mapping, Language Acquisition, Misconceptions
Serniclaes, Willy; Van Heghe, Sandra; Mousty, Philippe; Carre, Rene; Sprenger-Charolles, Liliane – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2004
Perceptual discrimination between speech sounds belonging to different phoneme categories is better than that between sounds falling within the same category. This property, known as ''categorical perception,'' is weaker in children affected by dyslexia. Categorical perception develops from the predispositions of newborns for discriminating all…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Auditory Discrimination, Phonemes, Neonates

Jacobsen, Terri Lomenick; Waters, Harriet Salatas – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
Second- and fourth-grade children viewed a cylindrical object in nine positions and identified the 90- , 180- , or 270-degree positions from a set of photographs. Perspectives in which the object differed from the child's view in both left-right and near-far dimensions were more difficult than perspectives that only transformed one dimension.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Mapping, Developmental Stages, Distance

Rack, John; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1994
Five-year-olds learned to associate three- or four-letter abbreviations, or cues, with spoken words, in which one of the letters in the cue corresponded to a phoneme that was articulated similarly or dissimilarly. Children found the phonetic cues easier to learn than control cues, suggesting that children are sensitive to the phonological and…
Descriptors: Abbreviations, Beginning Reading, Cognitive Mapping, Cues

Silverman, Irwin W.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
Varied conditions under which children aged four to five years matched the area of a rectangle with a given width or height to that of a square. Subjects matched one dimension of the rectangle to one side of the square suggesting that area matches seemed to be based on a side-matching strategy. (Author/AS)
Descriptors: Area, Cognitive Mapping, Dimensional Preference, Evaluative Thinking