NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Woodcock Language Proficiency…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 65 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ercenur Ünal; Kevser Kirbasoglu; Dilay Z. Karadöller; Beyza Sümer; Asli Özyürek – Cognitive Science, 2025
In spoken languages, children acquire locative terms in a cross-linguistically stable order. Terms similar in meaning to in and on emerge earlier than those similar to "front" and "behind," followed by "left" and "right." This order has been attributed to the complexity of the relations expressed by…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Cognitive Mapping, Spatial Ability, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jechun An; Dong-il Kim – Asia Pacific Education Review, 2024
This study aimed to evaluate the effects of intervention using graphic organizers on the cognitive and affective improvement of students with intellectual disability (ID), with learning disability (LD), without disability, and at-risk learners in Korea. A total of 49 peer-reviewed journals and dissertations for the last 20 years were included for…
Descriptors: Intervention, Instructional Materials, Foreign Countries, Meta Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Simms, Nina K.; Richland, Lindsey E. – Cognitive Science, 2019
Relational reasoning is a hallmark of human higher cognition and creativity, yet it is notoriously difficult to encourage in abstract tasks, even in adults. Generally, young children initially focus more on objects, but with age become more focused on relations. While prerequisite knowledge and cognitive resource maturation partially explains this…
Descriptors: Thinking Skills, Schemata (Cognition), Age Differences, Correlation
Simms, Nina; Richland, Lindsey – Grantee Submission, 2019
Relational reasoning is a hallmark of human higher cognition and creativity, yet it is notoriously difficult to encourage in abstract tasks, even in adults. Generally, young children initially focus more on objects, but with age become more focused on relations. While prerequisite knowledge and cognitive resource maturation partially explains this…
Descriptors: Thinking Skills, Schemata (Cognition), Age Differences, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Spanoudis, George; Demetriou, Andreas – Journal of Intelligence, 2020
The relations between the developing mind and developing brain are explored. We outline a theory of intellectual development postulating that the mind comprises four systems of processes (domain-specific, attention and working memory, reasoning, and cognizance) developing in four cycles (episodic, realistic, rule-based, and principle-based…
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Brain
Kim, Dan; Opfer, John E. – Grantee Submission, 2018
Young children's estimates of numerical magnitude increase approximately logarithmically with actual magnitude. The conventional interpretation of this finding is that children's estimates reflect an innate logarithmic encoding of number. A recent set of findings, however, suggests that logarithmic number-line estimates emerge via a dynamic…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Number Concepts, Concept Mapping, Numeracy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marie Arsalidou; Juan Pascual-Leone – npj Science of Learning, 2016
Neuroscience techniques provide an open window previously unavailable to the origin of thoughts and actions in children. Developmental cognitive neuroscience is booming, and knowledge from human brain mapping is finding its way into education and pediatric practice. Promises of application in developmental cognitive neuroscience rests however on…
Descriptors: Constructivism (Learning), Neurosciences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Samuelson, Larissa K.; Kucker, Sarah C.; Spencer, John P. – Cognitive Science, 2017
Theories of cognitive development must address both the issue of how children bring their knowledge to bear on behavior in-the-moment, and how knowledge changes over time. We argue that seeking answers to these questions requires an appreciation of the dynamic nature of the developing system in its full, reciprocal complexity. We illustrate this…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Vocabulary Development, Memory, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Möhring, Wenke; Newcombe, Nora S.; Frick, Andrea – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Spatial scaling is an important prerequisite for many spatial tasks and involves an understanding of how distances in different-sized spaces correspond. Previous studies have found evidence for such an understanding in preschoolers; however, the mental processes involved remain unclear. In the present study, we investigated whether children and…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Scaling, Preschool Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yoshida, Hanako; Hanania, Rima – First Language, 2013
One of the most prominent issues in early cognitive and linguistic development concerns how children figure out meanings of words from hearing them in context, since in many contexts there are multiple words and multiple potential referents for those words. Recent findings concerning on-line sentence comprehension suggest that, within the…
Descriptors: Competition, Vocabulary Development, Form Classes (Languages), Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Prasada, Sandeep; Hennefield, Laura; Otap, Daniel – Cognitive Science, 2012
We investigate the hypothesis that our conceptual systems provide two formally distinct ways of representing categories by investigating the manner in which lexical nominals (e.g., "tree," "picnic table") and phrasal nominals (e.g., "black bird," "birds that like rice") are interpreted. Four experiments found that lexical nominals may be mapped…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Cognitive Development, Classification, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berteletti, Ilaria; Lucangeli, Daniela; Zorzi, Marco – Cognition, 2012
The representation of numerical and non-numerical ordered sequences was investigated in children from preschool to grade 3. The child's conception of how sequence items map onto a spatial scale was tested using the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and new variants of the task designed to probe the representation of the alphabet…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Investigations, Preschool Education, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Johnson, Danette Ifert; Mrowka, Kaleigh – Communication Education, 2010
This investigation tests Wittrock's generative learning model as an explanation for the positive relationship found between quizzing and student performance in a number of studies. Results support the theory, suggesting that quizzes structured to include multiple levels of Bloom, Engelhart, Furst, Hill and Krathwohl's (1956) taxonomy, and thereby…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Learning Processes, Cognitive Mapping, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zabel, Jorg; Gropengiesser, Harald – Journal of Biological Education, 2011
The objective of this naturalistic study was to explore, model and visualise the learning progress of 13-year-old students in the domain of evolution theory. Data were collected under actual classroom conditions and with a sample size of 107 learners, which followed a teaching unit on Darwin's theory of natural selection. Before and after the…
Descriptors: Evolution, Theories, Science Education, Science Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Casler, Krista; Terziyan, Treysi; Greene, Kimberly – Cognitive Development, 2009
When children use objects like adults, are they simply tracking regularities in others' object use, or are they demonstrating a normatively defined awareness that there are right and wrong ways to act? This study provides the first evidence for the latter possibility. Young 2- and 3-year-olds (n = 32) learned functions of 6 artifacts, both…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Child Behavior, Object Manipulation, Feedback (Response)
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5