Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 15 |
Cognitive Processes | 15 |
Intelligence | 7 |
Intelligence Tests | 5 |
Children | 4 |
Cognitive Ability | 4 |
Cognitive Measurement | 4 |
Cognitive Tests | 4 |
Correlation | 4 |
Elementary Education | 4 |
Factor Analysis | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Intelligence | 15 |
Author
Cohen, Ronald L. | 2 |
Boomsma, Dorret I. | 1 |
Brooks, Penelope H. | 1 |
Brown, Ann L. | 1 |
Cunningham, Walter R. | 1 |
DeLoache, Judy S. | 1 |
DeVries, Rheta | 1 |
Feldman, Judith F. | 1 |
Glass, Gene V. | 1 |
Gohlke, B. | 1 |
Griffiths, Karen | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 15 |
Reports - Research | 12 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Cattell Culture Fair… | 1 |
Kit of Reference Tests for… | 1 |
Raven Progressive Matrices | 1 |
Woodcock Johnson Tests of… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rose, Susan A.; Feldman, Judith F.; Jankowski, Jeffery J.; Rossem, Ronan V. – Intelligence, 2008
Using data from a longitudinal study of preterms and full-terms, the present study examined the structure of infant cognition at 12 months, the extent to which five 12-month abilities (attention, processing speed, recognition, recall, and representational competence) mediated the relation from prematurity to mental development at 2-3 years, and…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Cognitive Processes
Kolata, Stefan; Light, Kenneth; Matzel, Louis D. – Intelligence, 2008
It has been established that both domain-specific (e.g. spatial) as well as domain-general (general intelligence) factors influence human cognition. However, the separation of these processes has rarely been attempted in studies using laboratory animals. Previously, we have found that the performances of outbred mice across a wide range of…
Descriptors: Experiments, Spatial Ability, Genetics, Animals
Masunaga, Hiromi; Kawashima, Ryuta; Horn, John L.; Sassa, Yuko; Sekiguchi, Atsushi – Intelligence, 2008
In our prior study the negative correlation between Topology, a behavioral measure of fluid reasoning, and adult age diminished with the increase in the level of expertise in a cognitively-demanding domain of expertise in the game of GO. The present fMRI study was designed to investigate neural substrates of Topology. The modified topology…
Descriptors: Expertise, Thinking Skills, Adult Development, Correlation
Keith, Timothy Z.; Reynolds, Matthew R.; Patel, Puja G.; Ridley, Kristen P. – Intelligence, 2008
Sex differences in the latent general and broad cognitive abilities underlying the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities were investigated for children, youth, and adults ages 6 through 59. A developmental, multiple indicator-multiple cause, structural equation model was used to investigate sex differences in latent cognitive abilities as…
Descriptors: Females, Males, Cognitive Ability, Structural Equation Models
van Leeuwen, Marieke; van den Berg, Stephanie M.; Hoekstra, Rosa A.; Boomsma, Dorret I. – Intelligence, 2007
The aim of this study was to identify promising endophenotypes for intelligence in children and adolescents for future genetic studies in cognitive development. Based on the available set of endophenotypes for intelligence in adults, cognitive tasks were chosen covering the domains of working memory, processing speed, and selective attention. This…
Descriptors: Memory, Adolescents, Reaction Time, Intelligence

Cohen, Ronald L.; Griffiths, Karen – Intelligence, 1987
To study age-related improvements in information processing, a release from proactive interference (PI) procedure was used with 144 children in conjunction with a running memory task. For class of item and acoustic similarity, evidence was found for PI release with age, but there was no evidence of a relationship between short-term memory (STM)…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Kempel, P.; Gohlke, B.; Klempau, J.; Zinsberger, P.; Reuter, M.; Hennig, J. – Intelligence, 2005
Based on stimulating findings suggesting that prenatal levels of steroids may influence cognitive functions, a study with N=40 healthy volunteers of both sexes was conducted. Prenatal levels of testosterone (T) were estimated by use of the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) which is supposed to be controlled by the same genes involved in…
Descriptors: Females, Cognitive Tests, Spatial Ability, Males

DeLoache, Judy S.; Brown, Ann L. – Intelligence, 1987
Memory-based searching was compared in 15 developmentally delayed and 24 normal two-year-olds. In a relatively easy memory task, individual differences were minimal, but searching for a plausible alternative location based on memory revealed marked individual differences and an important difference in the cognitive functioning of the delayed…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Disabilities

Brooks, Penelope H. – Intelligence, 1981
First and fifth graders in two IQ groupings reconstructed pictures which were variations on a prototypic picture. In subsequent recognition, children gave confidence ratings on the "oldness" of the pictures. Prototypes were recognized with most confidence. Younger and lower IQ children were less sure about noncases being "new".…
Descriptors: Ability Grouping, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education

Cunningham, Walter R. – Intelligence, 1980
The generality of ability factor structure in adulthood and old age was investigated. Data were analyzed for 198 young individuals (15-32 years), 156 younger old individuals (53-68 years), and 156 older old individuals (69-91). Variables were nine tests marking three ability factors. (Author/CTM)
Descriptors: Adult Development, Adults, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes

Keil, Frank C. – Intelligence, 1982
An approach to intelligence which emphasizes domain-specific constraints on knowledge structures is compared to information processing approaches. The evaluation of any cognitive ability as being intelligent crucially depends on prior specification of the formal constraints on the domains of knowledge from which that ability originates. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style

Glass, Gene V.; Stephens, Beth – Intelligence, 1980
Relationships among Piagetian reasoning assessments and standard measures of intelligence and achievement were determined in 1972 by Stephens, McLaughlin, Miller, and Glass (EJ 055 112). The data were reanalyzed by Humphreys and Parsons in 1979 (EJ 218 642). In reply, Glass and Stephens note fallacies in Humphreys' and Parsons' reasoning.…
Descriptors: Achievement Tests, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Measurement, Cognitive Processes

Humphreys, Lloyd G. – Intelligence, 1980
Stephens et al. (EJ 055 112) committed a serious methodological error in holding chronological age constant in their IQ measures and allowing it to vary in their Piagetian developmental measures. This error is unrelated to differences in factor rotation methods used by these authors and was not answered in their reply. (CTM)
Descriptors: Chronological Age, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Measurement, Cognitive Processes

Kohlberg, Lawrence; DeVries, Rheta – Intelligence, 1980
These authors cite their own study of the relationship between traditional measures of intelligence and Piagetian measures of cognitive development in support of Glass and Stephens' contention that there are important qualitative differences. They question Humphreys' and Parsons' conclusions on both substantive and factor theoretical grounds. (CTM)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Measurement, Cognitive Processes

Cohen, Ronald L.; Heath, Michele – Intelligence, 1990
The working memory hypothesis that the development of span in children is related to increasing proficiency in the use of an articulatory loop was tested in two studies with 120 Canadian children in grades 5 and 6 and 11 and 12. Some support was found for the hypothesis. (SLD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes