Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 227 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 334 |
Experimental Psychology | 104 |
Visual Stimuli | 101 |
Visual Perception | 97 |
Attention | 81 |
Reaction Time | 80 |
Task Analysis | 76 |
Spatial Ability | 68 |
Foreign Countries | 64 |
Cues | 62 |
Models | 54 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 334 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 323 |
Reports - Research | 264 |
Reports - Evaluative | 38 |
Reports - Descriptive | 13 |
Information Analyses | 4 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Reference Materials -… | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 88 |
Postsecondary Education | 15 |
Adult Education | 7 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Germany | 11 |
Israel | 9 |
United Kingdom | 7 |
Canada | 6 |
Netherlands | 5 |
Australia | 4 |
California | 4 |
Tennessee | 4 |
Massachusetts | 3 |
Switzerland | 3 |
United Kingdom (Scotland) | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Sweeny, Timothy D.; Haroz, Steve; Whitney, David – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Many species, including humans, display group behavior. Thus, perceiving crowds may be important for social interaction and survival. Here, we provide the first evidence that humans use ensemble-coding mechanisms to perceive the behavior of a crowd of people with surprisingly high sensitivity. Observers estimated the headings of briefly presented…
Descriptors: Group Behavior, Perception, Cognitive Processes, Motion
Jacoby, Oscar; Kamke, Marc R.; Mattingley, Jason B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
We have a remarkable ability to accurately estimate average featural information across groups of objects, such as their average size or orientation. It has been suggested that, unlike individual object processing, this process of "feature averaging" occurs automatically and relatively early in the course of perceptual processing,…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Orientation
Cosman, Joshua D.; Vecera, Shaun P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The ability to ignore task-irrelevant information and overcome distraction is central to our ability to efficiently carry out a number of tasks. One factor shown to strongly influence distraction is the perceptual load of the task being performed; as the perceptual load of task-relevant information processing increases, the likelihood that…
Descriptors: Attention, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Undergraduate Students
Tapia, Evelina; Breitmeyer, Bruno G.; Jacob, Jane; Broyles, Elizabeth C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Flanker congruency effects were measured in a masked flanker task to assess the properties of spatial attention during conscious and nonconscious processing of form, color, and conjunctions of these features. We found that (1) consciously and nonconsciously processed colored shape distractors (i.e., flankers) produce flanker congruency effects;…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Perception, Spatial Ability
Seli, Paul; Cheyne, James Allan; Smilek, Daniel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Mind wandering is a pervasive feature of human cognition often associated with the withdrawal of task-related executive control processes. Here, we explore the possibility that, in tasks requiring executive control to sustain consistent responding, moments of mind wandering could be associated with moments of increased behavioral variability. To…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Executive Function, Responses, Behavior
Lee, Chia-lin; Middleton, Erica; Mirman, Daniel; Kalenine, Solene; Buxbaum, Laurel J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Previous studies suggest that action representations are activated during object processing, even when task-irrelevant. In addition, there is evidence that lexical-semantic context may affect such activation during object processing. Finally, prior work from our laboratory and others indicates that function-based ("use") and structure-based…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Identification, Context Effect, Semantics
Mitra, Suvobrata; Knight, Alec; Munn, Alexandra – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Performing a cognitive task while maintaining upright stance can lead to increased or reduced body sway depending on tasks and experimental conditions. Because greater sway is commonly taken to indicate loosened postural control, and vice versa, the precise impact of cognitive load on postural stability has remained unclear. In much of the large…
Descriptors: Human Posture, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Coordination
Kamienkowski, Juan E.; Navajas, Joaquin; Sigman, Mariano – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
When presented with a sequence of visual stimuli in rapid succession, participants often fail to detect a second salient target, a phenomenon referred as the attentional blink (AB; Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992; Shapiro, Raymond, & Arnell, 1997). On the basis of a vast corpus of experiments, several cognitive theories suggest that the blink…
Descriptors: Evidence, Visual Stimuli, Eye Movements, Attention
Ko, Yao-Ting; Alsford, Toni; Miller, Jeff – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The forcefulness of key press responses was measured in stop-all and selective stopping versions of the stop-signal paradigm. When stop signals were presented too late for participants to succeed in stopping their responses, response force was nonetheless reduced relative to trials in which no stop signal was presented. This effect shows that…
Descriptors: Models, Inhibition, Responses, Cognitive Processes
Halvorson, Kimberly M.; Ebner, Herschel; Hazeltine, Eliot – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Why are dual-task costs reduced with ideomotor (IM) compatible tasks (Greenwald & Shulman, 1973; Lien, Proctor & Allen, 2002)? In the present experiments, we first examine three different measures of single-task performance (pure single-task blocks, mixed blocks, and long stimulus onset asynchrony [SOA] trials in dual-task blocks) and two…
Descriptors: Psychomotor Skills, Stimuli, Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes
Kristjansson, Arni; Oladottir, Berglind; Most, Steven B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Emotional stimuli often capture attention and disrupt effortful cognitive processing. However, cognitive processes vary in the degree to which they require effort. We investigated the impact of emotional pictures on visual search and on automatic priming of search. Observers performed visual search after task-irrelevant neutral or emotionally…
Descriptors: Psychological Patterns, Stimuli, Barriers, Cognitive Processes
Brown, Tracy L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The relationship between interference and facilitation effects in the Stroop task is poorly understood yet central to its implications. At question is the modal view that they arise from a single mechanism--the congruency of color and word. Two developments have challenged that view: (a) the belief that facilitation effects are fractionally small…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Color, Visual Perception, Correlation
Horoufchin, Himeh; Philipp, Andrea M.; Koch, Iring – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Decay of task-set activation, as commonly assumed in models of task switching, has been thought to be indexed by manipulating the response-to-cue interval (RCI) in a task-cuing paradigm. We propose an alternative account for RCI effects suggesting that episodic task retrieval is modulated by temporal distinctiveness, which we define as the ratio…
Descriptors: Repetition, Priming, Cues, Intervals
Neyedli, Heather F.; Welsh, Timothy N. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Previous research has revealed that people choose to aim toward an "optimal" endpoint when faced with a movement task with externally imposed payoffs. This optimal endpoint is modeled based on the magnitude of the payoffs and the probability of hitting the different payoff regions (endpoint variability). Endpoint selection, however, has only been…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Probability, Classification, Intervals
Anderson, David E.; Vogel, Edward K.; Awh, Edward – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Perceptual grouping can lead observers to perceive a multielement scene as a smaller number of hierarchical units. Past work has shown that grouping enables more elements to be stored in visual working memory (WM). Although this may appear to contradict so-called discrete resource models that argue for fixed item limits in WM storage, it is also…
Descriptors: Cluster Grouping, Cues, Mnemonics, Short Term Memory