NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)83
Source
Journal of Experimental…141
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 141 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sweeny, Timothy D.; Haroz, Steve; Whitney, David – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Many species, including humans, display group behavior. Thus, perceiving crowds may be important for social interaction and survival. Here, we provide the first evidence that humans use ensemble-coding mechanisms to perceive the behavior of a crowd of people with surprisingly high sensitivity. Observers estimated the headings of briefly presented…
Descriptors: Group Behavior, Perception, Cognitive Processes, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jacoby, Oscar; Kamke, Marc R.; Mattingley, Jason B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
We have a remarkable ability to accurately estimate average featural information across groups of objects, such as their average size or orientation. It has been suggested that, unlike individual object processing, this process of "feature averaging" occurs automatically and relatively early in the course of perceptual processing,…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tapia, Evelina; Breitmeyer, Bruno G.; Jacob, Jane; Broyles, Elizabeth C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Flanker congruency effects were measured in a masked flanker task to assess the properties of spatial attention during conscious and nonconscious processing of form, color, and conjunctions of these features. We found that (1) consciously and nonconsciously processed colored shape distractors (i.e., flankers) produce flanker congruency effects;…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Perception, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Linnell, Karina J.; Caparos, Serge – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Caparos and Linnell (2009, 2010) used a variable-separation flanker paradigm to show that (a) when cognitive load is low, increasing perceptual load causes spatial attention to focus and (b) when perceptual load is high, decreasing cognitive load causes spatial attention to focus. Here, we tested whether the effects of perceptual and cognitive…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Attention Control, Attention, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brown, Tracy L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The relationship between interference and facilitation effects in the Stroop task is poorly understood yet central to its implications. At question is the modal view that they arise from a single mechanism--the congruency of color and word. Two developments have challenged that view: (a) the belief that facilitation effects are fractionally small…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Color, Visual Perception, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Anderson, David E.; Vogel, Edward K.; Awh, Edward – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Perceptual grouping can lead observers to perceive a multielement scene as a smaller number of hierarchical units. Past work has shown that grouping enables more elements to be stored in visual working memory (WM). Although this may appear to contradict so-called discrete resource models that argue for fixed item limits in WM storage, it is also…
Descriptors: Cluster Grouping, Cues, Mnemonics, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hsieh, Po-Jang; Colas, Jaron T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The contents of working memory (WM) have predominantly been viewed as necessarily conscious. However, recent findings suggest otherwise. Here we investigate whether visual WM can represent subliminal stimuli, such that the positions of an invisible moving object can be extrapolated or learned about in terms of their task-relevant predictive power.…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Henderson, John M.; Nuthmann, Antje; Luke, Steven G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Recent research on eye movements during scene viewing has primarily focused on where the eyes fixate. But eye fixations also differ in their durations. Here we investigated whether fixation durations in scene viewing are under the direct and immediate control of the current visual input. Subjects freely viewed photographs of scenes in preparation…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Eye Movements, Photography, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Young, William; Rodger, Matthew; Craig, Cathy M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Many studies have examined the processes involved in recognizing types of human action through sound, but little is known about whether the physical characteristics of an action (such as kinetic and kinematic parameters) can be perceived and imitated from sound. Twelve young healthy adults listened to recordings of footsteps on a gravel path taken…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Kinetics, Physical Characteristics, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frings, Christian; Wentura, Dirk; Wuhr, Peter – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Research on the topic of distractor inhibition has used different empirical approaches to study how the human mind selects relevant information from the environment, and the results are controversially discussed. One key question that typically arises is how selection deals with the irrelevant information. We used a new selection task, in which…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention, Visual Perception, Inhibition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
White, Corey N.; Brown, Scott; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Two Bayesian observer models were recently proposed to account for data from the Eriksen flanker task, in which flanking items interfere with processing of a central target. One model assumes that interference stems from a perceptual bias to process nearby items as if they are compatible, and the other assumes that the interference is due to…
Descriptors: Tests, Bayesian Statistics, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cutting, James E.; Brunick, Kaitlin L.; Candan, Ayse – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
We selected 24 Hollywood movies released from 1940 through 2010 to serve as a film corpus. Eight viewers, three per film, parsed them into events, which are best termed subscenes. While watching a film a second time, viewers scrolled through frames and recorded the frame number where each event began. Viewers agreed about 90% of the time. We then…
Descriptors: Films, Color, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
O'Hearn, Kirsten; Franconeri, Steven; Wright, Catherine; Minshew, Nancy; Luna, Beatriz – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Evidence suggests that people with autism rely less on holistic visual information than typical adults. The current studies examine this by investigating core visual processes that contribute to holistic processing--namely, individuation and element grouping--and how they develop in participants with autism and typically developing (TD)…
Descriptors: Autism, Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tsubomi, Hiroyuki; Ikeda, Takashi; Osaka, Naoyuki – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Perceived brightness is well described by Stevens' power function (S. S. Stevens, 1957, On the psychophysical law, "Psychological Review", Vol. 64, pp. 153-181), with a power exponent of 0.33 (the cubic-root function of luminance). The power exponent actually varies across individuals, yet little is known about neural substrates underlying this…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Diagnostic Tests, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cornes, Katherine; Donnelly, Nick; Godwin, Hayward; Wenger, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The Thatcher illusion (Thompson, 1980) is considered to be a prototypical illustration of the notion that face perception is dependent on configural processes and representations. We explored this idea by examining the relative contributions of perceptual and decisional processes to the ability of observers to identify the orientation of two…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Churches, Human Body, Identification
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10