Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 64 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 76 |
Task Analysis | 76 |
Experimental Psychology | 23 |
Cues | 20 |
Foreign Countries | 20 |
Attention | 18 |
Visual Stimuli | 18 |
Models | 17 |
Spatial Ability | 17 |
Experiments | 16 |
Undergraduate Students | 16 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 76 |
Author
Miller, Jeff | 3 |
Verbruggen, Frederick | 3 |
Guest, Duncan | 2 |
Hommel, Bernhard | 2 |
Johnston, James C. | 2 |
Kluwe, Rainer H. | 2 |
Logan, Gordon D. | 2 |
Milliken, Bruce | 2 |
Oberauer, Klaus | 2 |
Peters, Alexandra | 2 |
Remington, Roger W. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 75 |
Reports - Research | 66 |
Reports - Evaluative | 6 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 27 |
Adult Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 6 |
Canada | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
Netherlands | 2 |
Tennessee | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
United Kingdom (London) | 2 |
Alabama | 1 |
Australia | 1 |
California | 1 |
France | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ko, Yao-Ting; Alsford, Toni; Miller, Jeff – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The forcefulness of key press responses was measured in stop-all and selective stopping versions of the stop-signal paradigm. When stop signals were presented too late for participants to succeed in stopping their responses, response force was nonetheless reduced relative to trials in which no stop signal was presented. This effect shows that…
Descriptors: Models, Inhibition, Responses, Cognitive Processes
Neyedli, Heather F.; Welsh, Timothy N. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Previous research has revealed that people choose to aim toward an "optimal" endpoint when faced with a movement task with externally imposed payoffs. This optimal endpoint is modeled based on the magnitude of the payoffs and the probability of hitting the different payoff regions (endpoint variability). Endpoint selection, however, has only been…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Probability, Classification, Intervals
Frischen, Alexandra; Ferrey, Anne E.; Burt, Dustin H. R.; Pistchik, Meghan; Fenske, Mark J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Affective evaluations of previously ignored visual stimuli are more negative than those of novel items or prior targets of attention or response. This has been taken as evidence that inhibition has negative affective consequences. But inhibition could act instead to attenuate or "neutralize" preexisting affective salience, predicting opposite…
Descriptors: Evidence, Visual Stimuli, Inhibition, Cognitive Processes
Kieffaber, Paul D.; Kruschke, John K.; Cho, Raymond Y.; Walker, Philip M.; Hetrick, William P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
The primary aim of the present research was to determine how "stimulus-set" and "response-set" components of task-set contribute to switch costs and conflict processing. Three experiments are described wherein participants completed an explicitly cued task-switching procedure. Experiment 1 established that task switches requiring a reconfiguration…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Conflict, Reaction Time, Stimuli
White, Corey N.; Brown, Scott; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Two Bayesian observer models were recently proposed to account for data from the Eriksen flanker task, in which flanking items interfere with processing of a central target. One model assumes that interference stems from a perceptual bias to process nearby items as if they are compatible, and the other assumes that the interference is due to…
Descriptors: Tests, Bayesian Statistics, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis
Braem, Senne; Verguts, Tom; Notebaert, Wim – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Cognitive control is responsible for adapting information processing in order to carry out tasks more efficiently. Contrasting global versus local control accounts, it has recently been proposed that control operates in an associative fashion, that is, by binding stimulus-response associations after detection of conflict (Verguts & Notebaert,…
Descriptors: Conflict, Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Prediction
Anderson, Brian A.; Folk, Charles L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Effective motor control involves both the execution of appropriate responses and the inhibition of inappropriate responses that are evoked by response-associated stimuli. The inhibition of a motor response has traditionally been characterized as either a voluntary act of cognitive control or a low-level perceptual bias arising from processes such…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Inhibition, Priming, Motor Reactions
Shaki, Samuel; Fischer, Martin H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
A recent cross-cultural comparison (Shaki, Fischer, & Petrusic, 2009) suggested that spatially consistent processing habits for words and numbers are a necessary condition for the spatial representation of numbers (Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes; SNARC effect). Here we reexamine the SNARC in Israelis who read text from right…
Descriptors: Cross Cultural Studies, Number Concepts, Numbers, Spatial Ability
Hommel, Bernhard; Fischer, Rico; Colzato, Lorenza S.; van den Wildenberg, Wery P. M.; Cellini, Cristiano – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Stressful situations, the aversiveness of events, or increases in task difficulty (e.g., conflict) have repeatedly been shown to be capable of triggering attentional control adjustments. In the present study we tested whether the particularity of an fMRI testing environment (i.e., EPI noise) might result in such increases of the cognitive control…
Descriptors: Diagnostic Tests, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Difficulty Level, Attention Control
Thompson, Sarah K.; Carlyon, Robert P.; Cusack, Rhodri – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Three experiments studied auditory streaming using sequences of alternating "ABA" triplets, where "A" and "B" were 50-ms tones differing in frequency by [delta]f semitones and separated by 75-ms gaps. Experiment 1 showed that detection of a short increase in the gap between a B tone and the preceding A tone, imposed on one ABA triplet, was better…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Attention, Intervals, Adults
Keetels, Mirjam; Vroomen, Jean – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The authors examined the effects of a task-irrelevant sound on visual processing. Participants were presented with revolving clocks at or around central fixation and reported the hand position of a target clock at the time an exogenous cue (1 clock turning red) or an endogenous cue (a line pointing toward 1 of the clocks) was presented. A…
Descriptors: Cues, Visual Perception, Cognitive Processes, Acoustics
Schmitz, Florian; Voss, Andreas – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
In four experiments, task-switching processes were investigated with variants of the alternating runs paradigm and the explicit cueing paradigm. The classical diffusion model for binary decisions (Ratcliff, 1978) was used to dissociate different components of task-switching costs. Findings can be reconciled with the view that task-switching…
Descriptors: Models, Cognitive Processes, Costs, Task Analysis
Dittrich, Kerstin; Stahl, Christoph – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Load theory predicts that concurrent cognitive load impairs selective attention. For visual stimuli, it has been shown that this impairment can be selective: Distraction was specifically increased when the stimulus material used in the cognitive load task matches that of the selective attention task. Here, we report four experiments that…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention Control, Visual Stimuli, Auditory Perception
Lee, Hyunkyu; Mozer, Michael C.; Kramer, Arthur F.; Vecera, Shaun P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
How is attention guided by past experience? In visual search, numerous studies have shown that recent trials influence responses to the current trial. Repeating features such as color, shape, or location of a target facilitates performance. Here we examine whether recent experience also modulates a more abstract dimension of attentional control,…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Development, Attention Control, Experience
Gellatly, Angus; Pilling, Michael; Carter, Wakefield; Guest, Duncan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Object substitution masking (OSM) is typically studied using a brief search display. The target item may be indicated by a cue/mask surrounding but not overlapping it. Report of the target is reduced when mask offset trails target offset rather than being simultaneous with it. We report 5 experiments investigating whether OSM can be obtained if…
Descriptors: Experiments, Cues, Adults, College Students