NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)1
Audience
Researchers2
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 83 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lynch, Sharon A.; Warner, Laverne – Childhood Education, 2012
Defining intelligence is a puzzle that has challenged educators and researchers for years. More recently, professionals are acknowledging that individuals possess many facets of intelligence and that learning is a complex combination of genetic factors, environmental influences, and life experiences that affect learning in unique ways (Salvia,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Multiple Intelligences, Educational Principles, Educational Practices
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moses, Louis J. – Child Development, 2001
Distinguishes two types of executive theories: (1) emergence accounts; and (2) expression accounts. Asserts that the meta-analytic findings reported by Wellman, Cross, and Watson (2001) are fully consistent with emergence accounts of theory of mind and do not entirely rule out expression accounts. (Author/KB)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Performance Factors
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mareschal, Denis; Johnson, Mark H. – Cognition, 2003
Tested 4-month-olds' memory for surface feature and location information following brief occlusions. Found that when target objects were images of female faces or monochromatic asterisks, infants increased looking times following changes in identity or color but not changes in location or combinations of feature and location. When objects were…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carlson, Stephanie M.; Moses, Louis J.; Hix, Hollie R. – Child Development, 1998
Three studies examined whether preschoolers' difficulties with deception and false belief arise from lack of inhibitory control rather than conceptual deficit. Found that 3-year olds deceived frequently under conditions requiring relatively low inhibitory control, but not high inhibitory control. Findings were not due to social intimidation, and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Deception, Inhibition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Templeton, Leslie M.; Wilcox, Sharon A. – Child Development, 2000
Investigated children's representational ability as a cognitive factor underlying the suggestibility of their eyewitness memory. Found that the eyewitness memory of children lacking multirepresentational abilities or sufficient general memory abilities (most 3- and 4-year-olds) was less accurate than eyewitness memory of those with…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Halford, Graeme S.; And Others – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1985
Concludes that strategies are not responsible for memory span development in children 7 through 13 years old. Running and fixed memory span tasks and a running probe task were administered to 38 children. The probe task showed age differences as great as with the fixed span task. Span was reduced by approximately half an item over all ages.…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Roth, Christopher – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1983
Finds that the usual adult superiority in speed of processing could be markedly reduced if children were given equivalent amounts of domain knowledge. The effect was domain specific; differences in knowledge affected processing rates in both knowledgeable adults and children to about the same extent. (Auther/RH)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wilcox, Teresa; Chapa, Catherine – Cognition, 2002
This study examined whether 9.5-month-olds could use featural information to individuate objects. Results suggest that infants categorize events involving opaque and transparent occluders as the same kind of situation and that infants are more likely to give evidence of individuation when they need to reason about one kind of event than when they…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kalish, Charles; Weissman, Michelle; Bernstein, Debra – Child Development, 2000
Three experiments assessed children's abilities to track behavioral, representational, and truth aspects of conventions. Three- and 4-year-olds recognized that conventional stipulations would change behavior, but not how stipulations might affect representations. Three- and 5-year-olds confused pretenses and conventions; 7-year-olds consistently…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Beliefs, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gouteux, Stephane; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2001
Eight experiments examined abilities of 3- to 4-year-olds to reorient themselves and locate a hidden object in an open circular space furnished with landmark objects. Findings showed that children failed to use geometric configuration of objects to reorient themselves. Children successfully located the object in relation to a geometric…
Descriptors: Adults, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wellman, Henry M.; Cross, David; Watson, Julanne – Child Development, 2001
Conducted meta-analysis to examine empirical inconsistencies and theoretical controversies concerning false-belief tasks and understanding about mental states. Found that a combined model including age, country of origin, and four task factors accounted for 55 percent of the variance in false-belief performance. Findings are consistent with…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Guajardo, Nicole R.; Turley-Ames, Kandi Jo – Cognitive Development, 2004
Two studies examined associations between theory of mind performance and counterfactual thinking using both antecedent and consequent counterfactual tasks. Moreover, the studies examined children's abilities to generate different types of counterfactual statements in terms of direction and structure. Participants were 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Thinking Skills, Cognitive Development, Learning Theories
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bertin, Evelin; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Examined three possible explanations for findings that infants detect textural discrepancies based on individual features more readily than on feature conjunctions. Found that none of the proposed factors could explain 5.5-month-olds' superior processing of featural over conjunction-based textural discrepancies. Findings suggest that in infancy,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hespos, Susan J.; Baillargeon, Renee – Cognition, 2001
Four experiments examined very young infants' expectations about containment events. Found that 2- to 3.5-month-olds recognized that objects could be lowered inside a container with an open but not a closed top. Three-and-a-half-month-olds realized that objects could not pass through the container's back wall and should have moved with it to a new…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Expectation, Infant Behavior
Sharp, Kay Colby; Waxman, Mindy – 1982
To investigate developmental differences, preschoolers' performance on four tasks frequently used to measure their understanding of causal and temporal relationships was studied. Based on an analysis of cognitive processes involved in recognition, completion, construction/seriation, and verbal explanation tasks, the prediction was made that…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages, Difficulty Level
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6