Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 54 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 132 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 139 |
Statistical Analysis | 139 |
Visual Stimuli | 86 |
Foreign Countries | 63 |
College Students | 45 |
Reaction Time | 40 |
Task Analysis | 37 |
Short Term Memory | 35 |
Pictorial Stimuli | 33 |
Undergraduate Students | 33 |
Cues | 30 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Dreisbach, Gesine | 3 |
Ball, B. Hunter | 2 |
Barrouillet, Pierre | 2 |
Brady, Nuala | 2 |
Braver, Todd S. | 2 |
Bugg, Julie M. | 2 |
Chuang, Hsueh-Hua | 2 |
Cleary, Laura | 2 |
De Deyne, Simon | 2 |
Fitzgerald, Michael | 2 |
Hutchison, Keith A. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 132 |
Reports - Research | 126 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 72 |
Postsecondary Education | 49 |
Elementary Education | 5 |
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Primary Education | 3 |
Grade 1 | 2 |
High Schools | 2 |
Junior High Schools | 2 |
Secondary Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Germany | 15 |
California | 5 |
Switzerland | 5 |
Missouri | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
North Carolina | 3 |
United Kingdom | 3 |
Belgium | 2 |
China | 2 |
Georgia | 2 |
Illinois | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Janczyk, Markus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Successful completion of any cognitive task requires selecting a particular action and the object the action is applied to. Oberauer (2009) suggested a working memory (WM) model comprising a declarative and a procedural part with analogous structures. One important assumption of this model is that both parts work independently of each other, and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Short Term Memory, Adolescents, Young Adults
Isolating Component Processes of Posterror Slowing with the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm
Steinhauser, Marco; Ernst, Benjamin; Ibald, Kevin W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Posterror slowing (PES) refers to an increased response time following errors. While PES has traditionally been attributed to control adjustments, recent evidence suggested that PES reflects interference. The present study investigated the hypothesis that control and interference represent 2 components of PES that differ with respect to their time…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Interference (Learning), Cognitive Processes, Classification
Langerock, Naomi; Vergauwe, Evie; Dirix, Nicolas; Barrouillet, Pierre – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Working memory, the system allowing for a simultaneous maintenance and processing of information, is typically conceived as a capacity limited system. A proposed method to transcend its standard maintenance capacity is to maintain multifeature objects, instead of isolated features. Several studies have shown that multifeature memory items are…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes, Hypothesis Testing, Visual Stimuli
Kühl, Tim; Stebner, Ferdinand; Navratil, Sabrina C.; Fehringer, Benedict C. O. F.; Münzer, Stefan – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2018
This research examined whether the informational advantage of an animation over a static picture (and over no visualizations as a control condition) can be compensated by presenting the information in the text that constitutes this informational advantage. In addition, it was investigated whether learners' spatial abilities acted as a compensator…
Descriptors: Animation, Visual Stimuli, Pictorial Stimuli, Spatial Ability
Dow, Gayle T. – Creativity Research Journal, 2015
Previous work has shown that the presence of examples may lead to cryptomnesia, or inadvertent plagiarism, on creative tasks. Various experiential and environmental attributes may magnify this finding. For instance, novices, with limited knowledge, may be more prone to inadvertently plagiarize examples, and increases in cognitive load may result…
Descriptors: Cheating, Plagiarism, Creativity, Cognitive Processes
Davis, Danielle K.; Abrams, Lise – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
When people read questions like "How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?", many mistakenly answer "2" despite knowing that Noah sailed the ark. This "Moses illusion" occurs when names share semantic features. Two experiments examined whether shared "visual" concepts (facial features)…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Semantics, Visual Stimuli, Interference (Learning)
Imbir, Kamil K. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2017
The aim of this study was to examine whether the valence and origin of emotional words can alter perception of ambiguous objects in terms of warmth versus competence, fundamental dimensions of social cognition. 60 individuals were invited into the study focusing on the limits of intuition. They were asked to try to guess the meaning of Japanese…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Language Usage, Affective Behavior, Competence
Parks, Colleen M.; Yonelinas, Andrew P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
It is often assumed that recollection is necessary to support memory for novel associations, whereas familiarity supports memory for single items. However, the levels of unitization framework assumes that familiarity can support associative memory under conditions in which the components of an association are unitized (i.e., treated as a single…
Descriptors: Memory, Familiarity, Cognitive Processes, Stimuli
Wirth, Robert; Janczyk, Markus; Kunde, Wilfried – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Actions aim to produce effects in the environment. To accomplish this properly, we not only have to recruit the appropriate motor patterns, but also we must be able to monitor whether an intended effect has ultimately been realized. Here, we investigated the impact of such effect monitoring on performance in multitasking situations: Multitasking…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Performance, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability
Ball, B. Hunter; Brewer, Gene A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
The present study implemented an individual differences approach in conjunction with response time (RT) variability and distribution modeling techniques to better characterize the cognitive control dynamics underlying ongoing task cost (i.e., slowing) and cue detection in event-based prospective memory (PM). Three experiments assessed the relation…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Memory
Eikmeier, Verena; Alex-Ruf, Simone; Maienborn, Claudia; Ulrich, Rolf – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Different lines of research suggest that our mental representations of time and space are linked, though the strength of this linkage has only recently been addressed for the front-back mental timeline (Eikmeier, Schröter, Maienborn, Alex-Ruf, & Ulrich, 2013). The present study extends this investigation to the left-right mental timeline. In…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Control Groups, Benchmarking, Time Perspective
Wang, Su-hua; Onishi, Kristine H. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2017
Infants' representations of physical events are surprisingly flexible. Brief exposure to one event can immediately enhance infants' representations of another event. The present experiments tested two potential mechanisms underlying this priming: enhanced encoding or improved retrieval. Five-month-olds saw a target block become hidden inside a…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Knowledge Representation, Observation
Peters, Benjamin; Rahm, Benjamin; Czoschke, Stefan; Barnes, Catherine; Kaiser, Jochen; Bledowski, Christoph – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Working memory (WM) enables a rapid access to a limited number of items that are no longer physically present. WM studies usually involve the encoding and retention of multiple items, while probing a single item only. Hence, little is known about how well multiple items can be reported from WM. Here we asked participants to successively report…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Recall (Psychology), Cues
Peterson, Dwight J.; Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
An important yet unresolved question regarding visual working memory (VWM) relates to whether or not binding processes within VWM require additional attentional resources compared with processing solely the individual components comprising these bindings. Previous findings indicate that binding of surface features (e.g., colored shapes) within VWM…
Descriptors: Attention, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Cognitive Processes
Williams, Carrick C.; Burkle, Kyle A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
To investigate the critical information in long-term visual memory representations of objects, we used occlusion to emphasize 1 type of information or another. By occluding 1 solid side of the object (e.g., top 50%) or by occluding 50% of the object with stripes (like a picket fence), we emphasized visible information about the object, processing…
Descriptors: Long Term Memory, Visual Perception, College Students, Pictorial Stimuli