Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Perspective Taking | 10 |
Task Analysis | 10 |
Spatial Ability | 4 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Attribution Theory | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Ability | 2 |
Cooperation | 2 |
Difficulty Level | 2 |
Listening Skills | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
American Educational Research… | 1 |
Brain and Cognition | 1 |
Child Development | 1 |
Cognition | 1 |
Psychological Record | 1 |
Social Development | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 9 |
Journal Articles | 8 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Accessibility versus Accuracy in Retrieving Spatial Memory: Evidence for Suboptimal Assumed Headings
Yerramsetti, Ashok; Marchette, Steven A.; Shelton, Amy L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Orientation dependence in spatial memory has often been interpreted in terms of accessibility: Object locations are encoded relative to a reference orientation that affords the most accurate access to spatial memory. An open question, however, is whether people naturally use this "preferred" orientation whenever recalling the space. We…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Spatial Ability, Memory, Familiarity
Tekkumru Kisa, Miray; Stein, Mary Kay – American Educational Research Journal, 2015
Students' opportunities to learn how to think are embedded in the instructional tasks with which they are invited to engage in the classroom. Prior research has revealed that the selection and use of cognitively demanding tasks does not guarantee high-level student thinking during their enactment. To address this challenge, we designed and…
Descriptors: Faculty Development, Video Technology, Science Instruction, Cognitive Processes
Surtees, Andrew D. R.; Apperly, Ian A. – Child Development, 2012
Children (aged 6-10) and adults (total N = 136) completed a novel visual perspective-taking task that allowed quantitative comparisons across age groups. All age groups found it harder to judge the other person's perspective when it differed from their own. This egocentric interference did not decrease with age, even though, overall, performance…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Perspective Taking, Children, Adults
Duran, Nicholas D.; Dale, Rick; Kreuz, Roger J. – Cognition, 2011
We explored perspective-taking behavior in a visuospatial mental rotation task that requires listeners to adopt an egocentric or "other-centric" frame of reference. In the current task, objects could be interpreted relative to the point-of-view of the listener (egocentric) or of a simulated partner (other-centric). Across three studies, we…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Spatial Ability, Social Environment, Perspective Taking
Fingelkurts, Andrew A.; Fingelkurts, Alexander A. – Brain and Cognition, 2011
Based on the theoretical analysis of self-consciousness concepts, we hypothesized that the spatio-temporal pattern of functional connectivity within the default-mode network (DMN) should persist unchanged across a variety of different cognitive tasks or acts, thus being task-unrelated. This supposition is in contrast with current understanding…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Cognitive Processes, Brain, Comparative Analysis
Villatte, Matthieu; Monestes, Jean-Louis; McHugh, Louise; Freixa i Baque, Esteve; Loas, Gwenole – Psychological Record, 2010
The current study assessed deictic relational responding in people with schizophrenia. A perspective-taking task and a mental states attribution task were employed with a sample of 15 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia and 15 age-matched controls. Results revealed poorer performance of participants with schizophrenia in responding in accordance…
Descriptors: Schizophrenia, Perspective Taking, Patients, Cognitive Development
Barber, Sarah J.; Franklin, Nancy; Naka, Makiko; Yoshimura, Hiroki – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Source monitoring is made difficult when the similarity between candidate sources increases. The current work examines how individual differences in social intelligence and perspective-taking abilities serve to increase source similarity and thus negatively impact source memory. Strangers first engaged in a cooperative storytelling task. On each…
Descriptors: Emotional Intelligence, Memory, Individual Differences, Perspective Taking
Gasser, Luciano; Keller, Monika – Social Development, 2009
The present study tested the hypothesis of the cognitively competent but morally insensitive bully. On the basis of teacher and peer ratings, 212 young elementary school children were selected and categorized as bullies, bully-victims, victims, and prosocial children. Children's perspective-taking skills were assessed using theory-of-mind tasks,…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Perspective Taking, Motivation, Bullying
Kraus, Marcy L. – 1984
The effects of age, task, and egocentric responding on visual-spatial perspective taking were studied among 41 preschool children between 3.0 and 5.9 years of age. Children were individually administered three perspective-taking measures: the upside-down/right-side-up task, a block task, and a picture box task, all previously described in the…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Egocentrism
Asher, Steven R.; Wigfield, Allan – 1980
A review of the research on developing children's referential communication ability reveals two approaches: the role-taking approach and the task-analytic approach. In the role-taking approach, researchers attempt to improve children's communication accuracy by training them to be more sensitive to listener needs. In the task-analytic approach,…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Processes, Communication Research, Communication Skills