Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 16 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 47 |
Learning Processes | 47 |
Young Children | 47 |
Cognitive Development | 18 |
Child Development | 10 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Early Childhood Education | 8 |
Metacognition | 7 |
Abstract Reasoning | 6 |
Cognitive Structures | 6 |
Developmental Stages | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Whiten, Andrew | 2 |
Asada, Minoru | 1 |
Astington, Janet Wilde | 1 |
Baker, Elizabeth | 1 |
Bauer, Patricia J. | 1 |
Boylan, Pamela | 1 |
Bu, Xiaomei | 1 |
Burnard, Pamela | 1 |
Burns, Dawn | 1 |
Cangelosi, Angelo | 1 |
Catherwood, Di | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 31 |
Reports - Research | 29 |
Opinion Papers | 6 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Books | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 7 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jiao, Xiaoyan; Zhang, Anqi; Bu, Xiaomei – Metacognition and Learning, 2023
Metacognition plays an important role in the development of young children. Recent studies have found that metacognition and executive function are independent but closely related. In this study, 55 children aged 4-5 years were selected as subjects, and a short-term longitudinal design was used to analyze the relationships among metacognition,…
Descriptors: Young Children, Metacognition, Mathematics Skills, Language Skills
Tomasetto, Carlo; Morsanyi, Kinga; Guardabassi, Veronica; O'Connor, Patrick A. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2021
Whereas some evidence exists that math anxiety may interfere with math performance from the very beginning of primary school, no study to date has attempted to investigate whether math anxiety may also interfere with early math learning (i.e., the encoding of new math knowledge) and not only with recalling already mastered contents in test…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Mathematics Anxiety, Elementary School Students, Interference (Learning)
Grzyb, Beata J.; Nagai, Yukie; Asada, Minoru; Cattani, Allegra; Floccia, Caroline; Cangelosi, Angelo – Developmental Science, 2019
Young children sometimes attempt an action on an object, which is inappropriate because of the object size--they make scale errors. Existing theories suggest that scale errors may result from immaturities in children's action planning system, which might be overpowered by increased complexity of object representations or developing teleofunctional…
Descriptors: Error Patterns, Young Children, Cognitive Processes, Semantics
Kim, Sunae; Paulus, Markus; Kalish, Chuck – Cognitive Science, 2017
Prior work shows that children selectively learn from credible speakers. Yet little is known how they treat information from non-credible speakers. This research examined to what extent and under what conditions children may or may not learn from problematic sources. In three studies, we found that children displayed trust toward previously…
Descriptors: Young Children, Trust (Psychology), Accuracy, Reliability
Bauer, Patricia J.; Varga, Nicole L.; King, Jessica E.; Nolen, Ayla M.; White, Elizabeth A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2015
Semantic knowledge can be extended in a variety of ways, including self-generation of new facts through integration of separate yet related episodes. We sought to promote integration and self-generation by providing "hints" to help 6-year-olds (Experiment 1) and 4-year-olds (Experiment 2) see the relevance of separate episodes to one…
Descriptors: Semantics, Children, Young Children, Control Groups
Jeste, Shafali S.; Kirkham, Natasha; Senturk, Damla; Hasenstab, Kyle; Sugar, Catherine; Kupelian, Chloe; Baker, Elizabeth; Sanders, Andrew J.; Shimizu, Christina; Norona, Amanda; Paparella, Tanya; Freeman, Stephanny F. N.; Johnson, Scott P. – Developmental Science, 2015
Statistical learning is characterized by detection of regularities in one's environment without an awareness or intention to learn, and it may play a critical role in language and social behavior. Accordingly, in this study we investigated the electrophysiological correlates of visual statistical learning in young children with autism…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Young Children, Visual Learning
Storkel, Holly L. – Journal of Child Language, 2011
Stoel-Gammon (this issue) states that "from birth to age 2 ; 6, the developing phonological system affects lexical acquisition to a greater degree than lexical factors affect phonological development" (this issue). This conclusion is based on a wealth of data; however, the available data are somewhat limited in scope, focusing on rather holistic…
Descriptors: Child Language, Vocabulary Development, Phonology, Young Children
Flynn, Emma; Whiten, Andrew – Child Development, 2012
In one of the first open diffusion experiments with young children, a tool-use task that afforded multiple methods to extract an enclosed reward and a child model habitually using one of these methods were introduced into different playgroups. Eighty-eight children, ranging in age from 2 years 8 months to 4 years 5 months, participated. Measures…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Socialization, Young Children, Verbal Ability
McGuigan, Nicola; Whiten, Andrew – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
We explored whether a rising trend to blindly "overcopy" a model's causally irrelevant actions between 3 and 5 years of age, found in previous studies, predicts a more circumspect disposition in much younger children. Children between 23 and 30 months of age observed a model use a tool to retrieve a reward from either a transparent or opaque…
Descriptors: Socialization, Toddlers, Young Children, Task Analysis
Smith, Anne B. – International Journal of Early Childhood, 2009
This ethnographic case study follows the trajectory of one child's learning disposition, reciprocity, and its relationship to the "learning architecture" of her early childhood and primary school learning environments, over eighteen months. Learning dispositions are coping strategies or habits of mind, and tendencies to respond to and select from…
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Young Children, Coping, Case Studies
Imai, Mutsumi; Kita, Sotaro; Nagumo, Miho; Okada, Hiroyuki – Cognition, 2008
Some words are sound-symbolic in that they involve a non-arbitrary relationship between sound and meaning. Here, we report that 25-month-old children are sensitive to cross-linguistically valid sound-symbolic matches in the domain of action and that this sound symbolism facilitates verb learning in young children. We constructed a set of novel…
Descriptors: Verbs, Japanese, Auditory Stimuli, Young Children
Salmon, Angela K. – Early Childhood Education Journal, 2008
Conscious of the interplay between nature and nurture in determining a child's individuality and success in life, the author embarked a group of teachers in an action research project towards nurturing a culture of thinking in young children. Considering the positive effects of routines in early learning experiences, the research consisted in…
Descriptors: Action Research, Young Children, Teacher Researchers, Thinking Skills
Coltman, Penny – Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development, 2006
This paper presents the findings of a study exploring the self-regulated use of mathematical metalanguage in the early years. Young children were filmed on two occasions in the naturalistic context of their eight foundation stage settings, including both nursery and reception classes. The children were engaged in mathematical activities designed…
Descriptors: Young Children, Metacognition, Developmental Stages, Cognitive Processes
Cook, Nancy – Instructor, 2006
Whether it is volcanoes or video games, teachers know that when children enjoy a subject, they are far more motivated to take charge of their education. What teachers are learning now is that offering high interest topics may be less important than offering kids challenging tasks--new problems to solve--that tap into the way children's brains are…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Student Motivation, Interviews, Cognitive Processes
Fisch, Shalom M. – 1999
Many studies have shown that children of various ages learn from educational television, but the studies have not explained how children extract and comprehend educational content from these television programs. This paper proposes a model, the "capacity model," that focuses on children's allocation of working memory resources while…
Descriptors: Audience Response, Cognitive Processes, Educational Television, Learning Processes