Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 13 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 13 |
College Students | 13 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Experimental Psychology | 5 |
Eye Movements | 4 |
Priming | 4 |
Visual Stimuli | 4 |
Cues | 3 |
Experiments | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
Problem Solving | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 13 |
Author
Aarts, Henk | 1 |
Al-Khindi, Timour | 1 |
Altmann, Erik M. | 1 |
Andrews, Mark | 1 |
Bijleveld, Erik | 1 |
Custers, Ruud | 1 |
Del Campo, Elena | 1 |
Dimigen, Olaf | 1 |
Epstude, Kai | 1 |
Foulsham, Tom | 1 |
Goldstone, Robert L. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 13 |
Reports - Research | 11 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 9 |
Postsecondary Education | 3 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Mayr, Ulrich; Kuhns, David; Rieter, Miranda – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2013
With the goal to determine the cognitive architecture that underlies flexible changes of control settings, we assessed within-trial and across-trial dynamics of attentional selection by tracking of eye movements in the context of a cued task-switching paradigm. Within-trial dynamics revealed a switch-induced, discrete delay in onset of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Eye Movements, Cues, Task Analysis
Foulsham, Tom; Kingstone, Alan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2013
Many modern theories propose that perceptual information is represented by the sensorimotor activity elicited by the original stimulus. Scanpath theory (Noton & Stark, 1971) predicts that reinstating a sequence of eye fixations will help an observer recognize a previously seen image. However, the only studies to investigate this are…
Descriptors: Memory, Theories, Eye Movements, Recognition (Psychology)
Ric, Francois; Muller, Dominique – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
This research shows that people can unconsciously initiate and follow arithmetic rules (e.g., addition). Participants were asked to detect whether a symbol was a digit. This symbol was preceded by 2 digits and a subliminal instruction: "add" or a control instruction. Participants were faster at identifying a symbol as a number when the…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, Problem Solving, Numbers
Bijleveld, Erik; Custers, Ruud; Aarts, Henk – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
When in pursuit of rewards, humans weigh the value of potential rewards against the amount of effort that is required to attain them. Although previous research has generally conceptualized this process as a deliberate calculation, recent work suggests that rudimentary mechanisms--operating without conscious intervention--play an important role as…
Descriptors: Priming, Rewards, Psychological Studies, Experiments
Inzlicht, Michael; Al-Khindi, Timour – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
Performance monitoring in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has largely been viewed as a cognitive, computational process devoid of emotion. A growing body of research, however, suggests that performance is moderated by motivational engagement and that a signal generated by the ACC, the error-related negativity (ERN), may partially reflect a…
Descriptors: Cues, Arousal Patterns, Motivation, Correlation
Hills, Thomas T.; Todd, Peter M.; Goldstone, Robert L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
The trade-off between exploration and exploitation is common to a wide variety of problems involving search in space and mind. The prevalence of this trade-off and its neurological underpinnings led us to propose domain-general cognitive search processes (Hills, Todd, & Goldstone, 2008). We propose further that these are consistent with the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Priming, Problem Solving, Goal Orientation
Kousta, Stavroula-Thaleia; Vigliocco, Gabriella; Vinson, David P.; Andrews, Mark; Del Campo, Elena – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
Although much is known about the representation and processing of concrete concepts, knowledge of what abstract semantics might be is severely limited. In this article we first address the adequacy of the 2 dominant accounts (dual coding theory and the context availability model) put forward in order to explain representation and processing…
Descriptors: Semantics, Cognitive Processes, Emotional Response, Concept Formation
Miele, David B.; Molden, Daniel C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
Previous research overwhelmingly suggests that feelings of ease people experience while processing information lead them to infer that their comprehension is high, whereas feelings of difficulty lead them to infer that their comprehension is low. However, the inferences people draw from their experiences of processing fluency should also vary in…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Intelligence, Inferences, Cognitive Processes
Dimigen, Olaf; Sommer, Werner; Hohlfeld, Annette; Jacobs, Arthur M.; Kliegl, Reinhold – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
Brain-electric correlates of reading have traditionally been studied with word-by-word presentation, a condition that eliminates important aspects of the normal reading process and precludes direct comparisons between neural activity and oculomotor behavior. In the present study, we investigated effects of word predictability on eye movements (EM)…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Sentences, Reading, Eye Movements
Mussweiler, Thomas; Epstude, Kai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2009
Comparisons are a ubiquitous process in information processing. Seven studies examine whether, how, and when comparative thinking increases the efficiency of judgment and choice. Studies 1-4 demonstrate that procedurally priming participants to engage in more vs. less comparison influences how they process information about a target. Specifically,…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Cognitive Processes, Foreign Countries, Efficiency
Patsenko, Elena G.; Altmann, Erik M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2010
Routine human behavior has often been attributed to plans--mental representations of sequences goals and actions--but can also be attributed to more opportunistic interactions of mind and a structured environment. This study asks whether performance on a task traditionally analyzed in terms of plans can be better understood from a "situated" (or…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Attention, Experimental Psychology, Memory
Lupyan, Gary – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2008
What are the consequences of calling things by their names? Six experiments investigated how classifying familiar objects with basic-level names (chairs, tables, and lamps) affected recognition memory. Memory was found to be worse for items that were overtly classified with the category name--as reflected by lower hit rates--compared with items…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Recognition (Psychology), Classification, Cognitive Processes
Topolinski, Sascha; Strack, Fritz – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2009
People can intuitively detect whether a word triad has a common remote associate (coherent) or does not have one (incoherent) before and independently of actually retrieving the common associate. The authors argue that semantic coherence increases the processing fluency for coherent triads and that this increased fluency triggers a brief and…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Semantics, Grammar, Probability