Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 18 |
Descriptor
College Students | 19 |
Experiments | 19 |
Probability | 19 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Stimuli | 5 |
Cues | 4 |
Experimental Psychology | 4 |
Accuracy | 3 |
Bayesian Statistics | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Context Effect | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Anderson, Frederick | 1 |
Angele, Bernhard | 1 |
Arndt, Jason | 1 |
Ashby, F. Gregory | 1 |
Best, Ryan M. | 1 |
Brancalion, Marcel L. | 1 |
Brandt, Andrew E. | 1 |
Brown, Scott D. | 1 |
Canaes, Larissa S. | 1 |
Cave, Kyle R. | 1 |
Chiesi, Francesca | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 18 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 13 |
Postsecondary Education | 7 |
Audience
Teachers | 1 |
Location
Michigan | 2 |
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
California | 1 |
California (Los Angeles) | 1 |
California (Santa Barbara) | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Indiana | 1 |
Massachusetts | 1 |
Minnesota | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Best, Ryan M.; Goldstone, Robert L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
Categorical perception (CP) effects manifest as faster or more accurate discrimination between objects that come from different categories compared with objects that come from the same category, controlling for the physical differences between the objects. The most popular explanations of CP effects have relied on perceptual warping causing…
Descriptors: Bias, Comparative Analysis, Models, College Students
Ashby, F. Gregory; Vucovich, Lauren E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Feedback is highly contingent on behavior if it eventually becomes easy to predict, and weakly contingent on behavior if it remains difficult or impossible to predict even after learning is complete. Many studies have demonstrated that humans and nonhuman animals are highly sensitive to feedback contingency, but no known studies have examined how…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Classification, Learning Processes, Associative Learning
Cortis Mack, Cathleen; Dent, Kevin; Ward, Geoff – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Three experiments examined the immediate free recall (IFR) of auditory-verbal and visuospatial materials from single-modality and dual-modality lists. In Experiment 1, we presented participants with between 1 and 16 spoken words, with between 1 and 16 visuospatial dot locations, or with between 1 and 16 words "and" dots with synchronized…
Descriptors: Input Output Analysis, Recall (Psychology), Auditory Stimuli, Verbal Stimuli
Lowder, Matthew W.; Ferreira, Fernanda – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigated the role of prediction in the processing of repair disfluencies (e.g., "The chef reached for some salt uh I mean some ketchup ..."). Experiment 1 showed that listeners were more likely to fixate a critical distractor item (e.g., "pepper") during the processing of repair…
Descriptors: Prediction, Evidence, Eye Movements, Experiments
Won, Bo-Yeong; Jiang, Yuhong V. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Recent empirical and theoretical work has depicted a close relationship between visual attention and visual working memory. For example, rehearsal in spatial working memory depends on spatial attention, whereas adding a secondary spatial working memory task impairs attentional deployment in visual search. These findings have led to the proposal…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Experiments, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Siegel, Lynn L.; Kahana, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Repeating an item in a list benefits recall performance, and this benefit increases when the repetitions are spaced apart (Madigan, 1969; Melton, 1970). Retrieved context theory incorporates 2 mechanisms that account for these effects: contextual variability and study-phase retrieval. Specifically, if an item presented at position "i" is…
Descriptors: Memory, Recall (Psychology), Context Effect, Cues
Angele, Bernhard; Laishley, Abby E.; Rayner, Keith; Liversedge, Simon P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
In a previous gaze-contingent boundary experiment, Angele and Rayner (2013) found that readers are likely to skip a word that appears to be the definite article "the" even when syntactic constraints do not allow for articles to occur in that position. In the present study, we investigated whether the word frequency of the preview of a…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Reading Processes, Word Recognition, Word Frequency
Hawkins, Guy; Brown, Scott D.; Steyvers, Mark; Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan – Cognitive Science, 2012
For decisions between many alternatives, the benchmark result is Hick's Law: that response time increases log-linearly with the number of choice alternatives. Even when Hick's Law is observed for response times, divergent results have been observed for error rates--sometimes error rates increase with the number of choice alternatives, and…
Descriptors: Bayesian Statistics, Reaction Time, Context Effect, Decision Making
Woolley, Thomas E.; du Sautoy, Marcus P. F. – School Science Review, 2011
In 2006, Marcus du Sautoy delivered the Royal Institution Christmas Lectures on the subject of mathematics. From this, a workshop was created using ideas illustrated in the programme. A group of mathematical students from the University of Oxford, known as Marcus' Marvellous Mathemagicians, were trained in this workshop and now travel to schools…
Descriptors: Workshops, Classroom Environment, Experiments, Mathematics
Jones, Bryan A.; Rachlin, Howard – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
A human social discount function measures the value to a person of a reward to another person at a given social distance. Just as delay discounting is a hyperbolic function of delay, and probability discounting is a hyperbolic function of odds-against, social discounting is a hyperbolic function of social distance. Experiment 1 obtained individual…
Descriptors: Rewards, Probability, Self Control, Altruism
Williams, Carrick C.; Pollatsek, Alexander; Cave, Kyle R.; Stroud, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
In 2 experiments, eye movements were examined during searches in which elements were grouped into four 9-item clusters. The target (a red or blue "T") was known in advance, and each cluster contained different numbers of target-color elements. Rather than color composition of a cluster invariantly guiding the order of search though…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Probability, Experimental Psychology, Experiments
Arndt, Jason – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Using 3 experiments, I examined false memory for encoding context by presenting Deese-Roediger-McDermott themes (Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995) in usual-looking fonts and by testing related, but unstudied, lure items in a font that was shown during encoding. In 2 of the experiments, testing lure items in the font used to study their…
Descriptors: Testing, Recognition (Psychology), Experiments, Memory
Hagmayer, York; Sloman, Steven A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2009
Causal considerations must be relevant for those making decisions. Whether to bring an umbrella or leave it at home depends on the causal consequences of these options. However, most current decision theories do not address causal reasoning. Here, the authors propose a causal model theory of choice based on causal Bayes nets. The critical ideas…
Descriptors: Causal Models, Inferences, Decision Making, Intervention
The Conjunction Fallacy: A Derived Stimulus Relations Conceptualization and Demonstration Experiment
Gaynor, Scott T.; Washio, Yukiko; Anderson, Frederick – Psychological Record, 2007
The conjunction fallacy (CF) comes about when the occurrence of two events is rated as more likely than either in isolation. A typical participant in a CF study is presented with a description of a hypothetical individual (i.e., a compound sample stimulus) and then asked to make judgments as to the likelihood that that person engages in a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Probability, Experiments, College Students
Morsanyi, Kinga; Primi, Caterina; Chiesi, Francesca; Handley, Simon – Contemporary Educational Psychology, 2009
In three studies we looked at two typical misconceptions of probability: the representativeness heuristic, and the equiprobability bias. The literature on statistics education predicts that some typical errors and biases (e.g., the equiprobability bias) increase with education, whereas others decrease. This is in contrast with reasoning theorists'…
Descriptors: Heuristics, Logical Thinking, Psychology, Statistics
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2