Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
College Students | 8 |
Stimuli | 8 |
Task Analysis | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 5 |
Classification | 3 |
Statistical Analysis | 3 |
Attention Control | 2 |
Correlation | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Experimental Psychology | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Ashby, F. Gregory | 2 |
Bowman, Howard | 1 |
Buetti, Simona | 1 |
Crooks, Anna | 1 |
Dankovicova, Jana | 1 |
De Los Reyes, Andres | 1 |
Dreisbach, Gesine | 1 |
Fischer, Rico | 1 |
Gottschalk, Caroline | 1 |
House, Jill | 1 |
Jones, Katie | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 7 |
Postsecondary Education | 3 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rosedahl, Luke A.; Ashby, F. Gregory – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
In rule-based (RB) category-learning tasks, the optimal strategy is a simple explicit rule, whereas in information-integration (II) tasks, the optimal strategy is impossible to describe verbally. This study investigates the effects of two different category properties on learning difficulty in category learning tasks--namely, linear separability…
Descriptors: Classification, Learning, College Students, Difficulty Level
Ashby, F. Gregory; Vucovich, Lauren E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Feedback is highly contingent on behavior if it eventually becomes easy to predict, and weakly contingent on behavior if it remains difficult or impossible to predict even after learning is complete. Many studies have demonstrated that humans and nonhuman animals are highly sensitive to feedback contingency, but no known studies have examined how…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Classification, Learning Processes, Associative Learning
Fischer, Rico; Gottschalk, Caroline; Dreisbach, Gesine – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Performing 2 highly similar tasks at the same time requires an adaptive regulation of cognitive control to shield prioritized primary task processing from between-task (cross-talk) interference caused by secondary task processing. In the present study, the authors investigated how implicitly and explicitly delivered information promotes the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention Control, Context Effect, Task Analysis
Olds, Justin M.; Westerman, Deanne L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Stimuli that are processed fluently tend to be regarded as more familiar and are more likely to be classified as old on a recognition test compared with less fluent stimuli. Recently it was shown that the standard relationship between fluency and positive recognition judgments can be reversed if participants are trained that previously studied…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Recall (Psychology), Recognition (Psychology), Feedback (Response)
Kundey, Shannon M. A.; De Los Reyes, Andres; Taglang, Chelsea M. – Educational Psychology, 2011
College students frequently experience inattentive and hyperactive concerns. In multiple independent samples and three randomised experiments, we examined multiple versions of a short performance-based measure translated from basic research on how organisms learn sequential stimuli patterns when such patterns are interleaved with information that…
Descriptors: College Students, Stimuli, Student Evaluation, Cognitive Processes
Buetti, Simona; Kerzel, Dirk – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
In the Simon effect, participants make a left or right keypress in response to a nonspatial attribute (e.g., color) that is presented on the left or right. Reaction times (RTs) increase when the response activated by the irrelevant stimulus location and the response retrieved by instruction are in conflict. The authors measured RTs and movement…
Descriptors: Conflict, Programming, Cognitive Processes, Stimuli
Wyble, Brad; Bowman, Howard; Potter, Mary C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
Transient attention to a visually salient cue enhances processing of a subsequent target in the same spatial location between 50 to 150 ms after cue onset (K. Nakayama & M. Mackeben, 1989). Do stimuli from a categorically defined target set, such as letters or digits, also generate transient attention? Participants reported digit targets among…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Cues
Dankovicova, Jana; House, Jill; Crooks, Anna; Jones, Katie – Language and Speech, 2007
Few attempts have been made to look systematically at the relationship between musical and intonation analysis skills, a relationship that has been to date suggested only by informal observations. Following Mackenzie Beck (2003), who showed that musical ability was a useful predictor of general phonetic skills, we report on two studies…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Music Education, Music, Intonation