NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Porter, Christin L.; Jones, Blake L.; Evans, Cortney A.; Robinson, Clyde C. – Infancy, 2009
This study examined both differential patterns and the stability of infants' (N = 70) distress reactivity across mother and stranger arm-restraint conditions when infants were 6 and 9 months of age. Reactivity measures included observational variables for the rise, intensity, and duration of infant distress as well as motor activities associated…
Descriptors: Mothers, Infants, Stranger Reactions, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jeon, Hana; Moulson, Margaret C.; Fox, Nathan; Zeanah, Charles; Nelson, Charles A., III – Infancy, 2010
The current study examined the effects of institutionalization on the discrimination of facial expressions of emotion in three groups of 42-month-old children. One group consisted of children abandoned at birth who were randomly assigned to Care-as-Usual (institutional care) following a baseline assessment. Another group consisted of children…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Familiarity, Parents, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kaldy, Zsuzsa; Blaser, Erik – Infancy, 2009
What kind of featural information do infants rely on when they are trying to recognize a previously seen object? The question of whether infants use certain features (e.g., shape or color) more than others (e.g., luminance) can only be studied legitimately if visual salience is controlled, as the magnitude of feature values--how noticeable and…
Descriptors: Age, Identification, Infants, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brown, Josephine V.; Bakeman, Roger; Sampers, Jackie S.; Korner, Anneliese F.; Constantinou, Janet C.; Anand, K. J. S. – Infancy, 2008
In spite of numerous recent outcome studies of extremely low birth weight (ELBW) infants, no data exist on their development prior to term. In this study we traced and compared the neurobehavioral development of 251 ELBW (less than 1,000 g) and 240 low birth weight (LBW; 1,000 g-2,500 g) preterms born between 1995 and 2004 from 32 to 37 weeks…
Descriptors: Body Weight, Motor Development, Premature Infants, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cote, Linda R.; Bornstein, Marc H.; Haynes, O. Maurice; Bakeman, Roger – Infancy, 2008
Cultural variation in durations, relations, and contingencies of mother-infant person- and object-directed behaviors were examined for 121 nonmigrant Latino mother-infant dyads in South America, Latina immigrants from South America and their infants living in the United States, and European American mother-infant dyads. Nonmigrant Latina mothers…
Descriptors: Mothers, Infants, Parent Child Relationship, Cultural Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Newman, Rochelle S.; Hussain, Isma – Infancy, 2006
Although a large literature discusses infants' preference for infant-directed speech (IDS), few studies have examined how this preference might change over time or across listening situations. The work reported here compares infants' preference for IDS while listening in a quiet versus a noisy environment, and across 3 points in development: 4.5…
Descriptors: Infants, Speech, Listening, Auditory Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Venezia, Meaghan; Messinger, Daniel S.; Thorp, Danielle; Mundy, Peter – Infancy, 2004
When do infants begin to communicate positive affect about physical objects to their social partners? We examined developmental changes in the timing of smiles during episodes of initiating joint attention that involved an infant gazing between an object and a social partner. Twenty-six typically developing infants were observed at 8, 10, and 12…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Infants, Developmental Stages, Interpersonal Communication