NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 28 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Litt, Robin A.; de Jong, Peter F.; van Bergen, Elsje; Nation, Kate – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Recent research suggests that visual-verbal paired associate learning (PAL) may tap a crossmodal associative learning mechanism that plays a distinct role in reading development. However, evidence from children with dyslexia indicates that deficits in visual-verbal PAL are strongly linked to the verbal demands of the task. The primary aim of this…
Descriptors: Dyslexia, Paired Associate Learning, Structural Equation Models, Reading Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Betsch, Tilmann; Lang, Anna – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
We studied risky choices in preschoolers, elementary schoolers, and adults using an information board paradigm crossing two options with two cues that differ in their probability of making valid predictions (p = 0.50 vs. p = 0.83). We also varied the presence of normatively irrelevant information. Choice patterns indicate that preschoolers were…
Descriptors: Prediction, Probability, Preschool Children, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Steinbeis, Nikolaus; Singer, Tania – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Social comparison can elicit emotions such as envy, which can affect social interactions. The emergence and development of such social emotions through ontogeny, and their influence on social interaction, are unknown. We tested 182 children from 7 to 13 years of age with a novel monetary reward-and-punishment task measuring envy and Schadenfreude…
Descriptors: Resource Allocation, Children, Interpersonal Relationship, Prosocial Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huang-Pollock, Cynthia L.; Maddox, W. Todd; Karalunas, Sarah L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
We present two studies that examined developmental differences in the implicit and explicit acquisition of category knowledge. College-attending adults consistently outperformed school-age children on two separate information-integration paradigms due to children's more frequent use of an explicit rule-based strategy. Accuracy rates were also…
Descriptors: Classification, Age Differences, Individual Development, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sloutsky, Vladimir M.; Fisher, Anna V. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Linguistic labels affect inductive generalization; however, the mechanism underlying these effects remains unclear. According to one similarity-based model, SINC (similarity, induction, naming, and categorization), early in development labels are features of objects contributing to the overall similarity of compared entities, with early induction…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Infants, Logical Thinking, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riggs, Kevin J.; Simpson, Andrew; Potts, Thomas – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Visual short-term memory (VSTM) research suggests that the adult capacity is limited to three or four multifeature object representations. Despite evidence supporting a developmental increase in capacity, it remains unclear what the unit of capacity is in children. The current study employed the change detection paradigm to investigate both the…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Memorization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Defever, Emmy; Sasanguie, Delphine; Gebuis, Titia; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
How people process and represent magnitude has often been studied using number comparison tasks. From the results of these tasks, a comparison distance effect (CDE) is generated, showing that it is easier to discriminate two numbers that are numerically further apart (e.g., 2 and 8) compared with numerically closer numbers (e.g., 6 and 8).…
Descriptors: Models, Mathematics Tests, Kindergarten, Grade 6
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gao, Xiaoqing; Maurer, Daphne; Nishimura, Mayu – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
We explored the perceptual structure of facial expressions of six basic emotions, varying systematically in intensity, in adults and children aged 7 and 14 years. Multidimensional scaling suggested that three- or four-dimensional structures were optimal for all groups. Two groups of adults demonstrated nearly identical structure, which had…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Models, Multidimensional Scaling, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hopper, Lydia M.; Flynn, Emma G.; Wood, Lara A. N.; Whiten, Andrew – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
In the first of two experiments, we demonstrate the spread of a novel form of tool use across 20 "cultural generations" of child-to-child transmission. An experimentally seeded technique spread with 100% fidelity along twice as many "generations" as has been investigated in recent exploratory "diffusion" experiments of this type. This contrasted…
Descriptors: Socialization, Population Distribution, Imitation, Observational Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haikio, Tuomo; Bertram, Raymond; Hyona, Jukka; Niemi, Pekka – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
By means of the moving window paradigm, we examined how many letters can be identified during a single eye fixation and whether this letter identity span changes as a function of reading skill. The results revealed that 8-year-old Finnish readers identify approximately 5 characters, 10-year-old readers identify approximately 7 characters, and…
Descriptors: Age, Models, Eye Movements, Decoding (Reading)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Chapman, Michael – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1989
Argues that the resources and response competition models discussed by Brainerd and Reyna (in this issue) may not be mutually exclusive, but instead may model different aspects of performance. The problem is not to decide between the two models in general, but rather to determine which aspects of performance are best explained by each. (RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Memory, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pascual-Leone, Juan; Sparkman, Eric – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1980
Epistemological and methodological differences which underlie empiricist v rationalist science are described. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Epistemology, Models, Research Methodology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Spiker, Charles C.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1972
Rejoinder to PS 502 062 which in turn is an alternative analysis of authors' study presented in PS 502 061. (MB)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Goodness of Fit, Learning Theories, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pascual-Leone, Juan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2000
Compares and contrasts working memory theory of Baddeley and theory of constructive operators of Pascual- Leone. Concludes that although the theory of constructive operators is complementary with working memory theory (explains developmental and individual differences that working memory theory cannot), the converse is not true; theory of…
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carter, Philip; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1983
Two experiments studied nine-year-olds, l3-year-olds, and adults in their encoding of two kinds of stimuli taken from a psychometric measure of spatial aptitude. The first experiment used letter-like stimuli; the second employed multi-element flags. (CI)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Algorithms, Children
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2