Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Comparative Analysis | 10 |
Task Analysis | 10 |
Memory | 5 |
Semantics | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
Phonology | 3 |
Pictorial Stimuli | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Aging (Individuals) | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 10 |
Author
Balota, David A. | 1 |
Budd, Mary-Jane | 1 |
Craik, Fergus I. M. | 1 |
Emmorey, Karen | 1 |
Ferreira, Victor S. | 1 |
Foster, Nathaniel L. | 1 |
Gollan, Tamar H. | 1 |
Griffiths, Yvonne | 1 |
Hanley, J. Richard | 1 |
Jones, Lara L. | 1 |
Luo, Lin | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jones, Lara L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Mediated priming refers to the faster word recognition of a target (e.g., milk) following presentation of a prime (e.g., pasture) that is related indirectly via a connecting "mediator" (e.g., cow). Association strength may be an important factor in whether mediated priming occurs prospectively (with target activation prior to its presentation) or…
Descriptors: Priming, Word Recognition, Language Processing, Cues
Perfors, Amy – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
The Less is More hypothesis suggests that one reason adults and children differ in their ability to learn language is that they also differ in other cognitive capacities. According to one version of this hypothesis, children's relatively poor memory may make them more likely to regularize inconsistent input (Hudson Kam & Newport, 2005, 2009). This…
Descriptors: Memory, Adults, Children, Cognitive Ability
Emmorey, Karen; Petrich, Jennifer A. F.; Gollan, Tamar H. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Bilinguals who are fluent in American Sign Language (ASL) and English often produce "code-blends"--simultaneously articulating a sign and a word while conversing with other ASL-English bilinguals. To investigate the cognitive mechanisms underlying code-blend processing, we compared picture-naming times (Experiment 1) and semantic categorization…
Descriptors: Speech, Language Processing, American Sign Language, Semantics
Budd, Mary-Jane; Hanley, J. Richard; Griffiths, Yvonne – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
This study investigated whether Foygel and Dell's (2000) interactive two-step model of speech production could simulate the number and type of errors made in picture-naming by 68 children of elementary-school age. Results showed that the model provided a satisfactory simulation of the mean error profile of children aged five, six, seven, eight and…
Descriptors: Speech, Phonology, Semantics, Children
Sahakyan, Lili; Foster, Nathaniel L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Performing action phrases (subject-performed tasks, SPTs) leads to better memory than verbal learning instructions (verbal tasks, VTs). In Experiments 1-3, the list-method directed forgetting design produced equivalent directed forgetting impairment for VTs and SPTs; however, directed forgetting enhancement emerged only for VTs, but not SPTs.…
Descriptors: Test Items, Verbal Learning, Serial Ordering, Memory
Trueswell, John C.; Papafragou, Anna – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
What role does language play during attention allocation in perceiving and remembering events? We recorded adults' eye movements as they studied animated motion events for a later recognition task. We compared native speakers of two languages that use different means of expressing motion (Greek and English). In Experiment 1, eye movements revealed…
Descriptors: Language Usage, Eye Movements, Attention Control, Motion
Luo, Lin; Craik, Fergus I. M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Three experiments were conducted to examine the effect of specificity at retrieval on the size of age differences in recollection. Participants encoded words in different contexts and were given recognition tests. Some of the test lists were constructed so that participants had to recollect specific aspects of the initial encoding events, whereas…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Age Differences, Memory, Recognition (Psychology)
Meade, Michelle L.; Watson, Jason M.; Balota, David A.; Roediger, Henry L., III – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
The nature of persisting spreading activation from list presentation in eliciting false recognition in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm was examined in two experiments. We compared the time course of semantic priming in the lexical decision task (LDT) and false alarms in speeded recognition under identical study and test conditions. The…
Descriptors: Test Items, Semantics, Models, Cognitive Processes
Slevc, L. Robert; Ferreira, Victor S. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
The "perceptual loop theory" of speech monitoring (Levelt, 1983) claims that inner and overt speech are monitored by the comprehension system, which detects errors by comparing the comprehension of formulated utterances to originally intended utterances. To test the perceptual loop monitor, speakers named pictures and sometimes attempted to halt…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Inner Speech (Subvocal), Phonology, Semantics
Ratcliff, Roger; Thapar, Anjali; McKoon, Gail – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
The effects of aging on response time were examined in a recognition memory experiment with young, college age subjects and older, 60-75 year old subjects. The older subjects were slower than the young subjects but almost as accurate. Ratcliff's (1978) diffusion model was fit to the data and it provided a good account of response times, their…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Aging (Individuals), Reaction Time, College Students