Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 8 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 31 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 34 |
Comparative Analysis | 34 |
Nonverbal Communication | 34 |
Children | 13 |
Autism | 11 |
Pervasive Developmental… | 11 |
Infants | 10 |
Foreign Countries | 8 |
Young Children | 8 |
Adults | 7 |
Eye Movements | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Moulson, Margaret C. | 2 |
Vida, Mark D. | 2 |
A. Delcenserie | 1 |
Alibali, Martha W. | 1 |
Allen, Jillean | 1 |
Apostu, A. | 1 |
Barthomeuf, Laetitia | 1 |
Bavin, Edith | 1 |
Bertenthal, Bennett I. | 1 |
Bolte, Sven | 1 |
Boyer, Ty W. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 34 |
Reports - Research | 32 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 2 |
Romania | 2 |
Canada | 1 |
Florida | 1 |
France | 1 |
Hong Kong | 1 |
New York | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lister, Casey J.; Burtenshaw, Tiarn; Walker, Bradley; Ohan, Jeneva L.; Fay, Nicolas – Child Development, 2021
Naturalistic studies show that children can create language-like communication systems in the absence of conventional language. However, experimental evidence is mixed. We address this discrepancy using an experimental paradigm that simulates naturalistic sign creation. Specifically, we tested if a sample of 6- to 12-year-old children (52 girls…
Descriptors: Case Studies, Sign Language, Nonverbal Communication, Comparative Analysis
A. Delcenserie; F. Genesee; F. Champoux – Developmental Science, 2024
Recent evidence suggests that deaf children with CIs exposed to nonnative sign language from hearing parents can attain age-appropriate vocabularies in both sign and spoken language. It remains to be explored whether deaf children with CIs who are exposed to early nonnative sign language, but only up to implantation, also benefit from this input…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Linguistic Input, Phonology, Nonverbal Communication
Schaller, Ulrich M.; Rauh, Reinhold – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2017
We tested social cognition abilities of adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and neurotypically developed peers (NTD). A multi-faceted test-battery including facial emotion categorization (FEC), classical false belief tasks (FBT), and complex social cognition (SC), yielded significantly lower accuracy rates for FEC and complex SC tasks…
Descriptors: Social Cognition, Adolescents, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Schwichtenberg, AJ; Kellerman, Ashleigh M.; Young, Gregory S.; Miller, Meghan; Ozonoff, Sally – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2019
Mother-infant interactions are a proximal process in early development and may be especially salient for children who are at risk for social difficulties (i.e. infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder). To inform how indices of maternal behaviors may improve parent-mediated interventions designed to mitigate autism spectrum…
Descriptors: Mothers, Infants, Parent Child Relationship, At Risk Persons
Dubey, Indu; Ropar, Danielle; de C. Hamilton, Antonia F. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2017
The recently proposed Social Motivation theory (Chevallier et al., Trends in cognitive sciences 16(4):231-239, 2012) suggests that social difficulties in Autism Spectrum Condition (ASC) might be caused by a difference in the motivation to engage with other people. Here we compared adolescents with (N = 31) and without (N = 37) ASC on the…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Adolescents, Comparative Analysis
Ryan, Christian; Furley, Philip; Mulhall, Kathleen – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2016
Typically developing children are able to judge who is winning or losing from very short clips of video footage of behaviour between active match play across a number of sports. Inferences from "thin slices" (short video clips) allow participants to make complex judgments about the meaning of posture, gesture and body language. This…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Video Technology, Nonverbal Communication
Wray, Charlotte; Norbury, Courtenay Frazier – First Language, 2018
Parent-child interaction plays a crucial role in early language acquisition. In young typically developing children, direct and indirect relationships between parent gesture, child gesture and child language have been observed. Far less is known about these relationships in atypical language development. The present study investigated parent…
Descriptors: Parent Child Relationship, Nonverbal Communication, Language Acquisition, Problem Solving
Cognitive and Adaptive Advantages of Growth Hormone Treatment in Children with Prader-Willi Syndrome
Dykens, Elisabeth M.; Roof, Elizabeth; Hunt-Hawkins, Hailee – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2017
Background: People with Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS) typically have mild to moderate intellectual deficits, compulsivity, hyperphagia, obesity, and growth hormone deficiencies. Growth hormone treatment (GHT) in PWS has well-established salutatory effects on linear growth and body composition, yet cognitive benefits of GHT, seen in other patient…
Descriptors: Genetic Disorders, Therapy, Cognitive Ability, Adjustment (to Environment)
Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Boyer, Ty W.; Harding, Samuel – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Infants' understanding of a pointing gesture represents a major milestone in their communicative development. The current consensus is that infants are not capable of following a pointing gesture until 9-12 months of age. In this article, we present evidence from 4- and 6-month-old infants challenging this conclusion. Infants were tested with…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Eye Movements, Attention
Mainela-Arnold, Elina; Alibali, Martha W.; Hostetter, Autumn B.; Evans, Julia L. – International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 2014
Background: Previous research suggests that speakers are especially likely to produce manual communicative gestures when they have relative ease in thinking about the spatial elements of what they are describing, paired with relative difficulty organizing those elements into appropriate spoken language. Children with specific language impairment…
Descriptors: Language Impairments, Nonverbal Communication, Communication Skills, Expressive Language
Oner, Pinar; Oner, Ozgur; Munir, Kerim – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2014
We compared ratings on the Three-Item Direct Observation Screen test for autism spectrum disorders completed by pediatric residents with the Social Communication Questionnaire parent reports as an augmentative tool for improving autism spectrum disorder screening performance. We examined three groups of children (18-60 months) comparable in age…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Observation, Screening Tests
Yow, W. Quin – Journal of Child Language, 2015
Young children typically do not use order-of-mention to resolve ambiguous pronouns, but may do so if given additional cues, such as gestures. Additionally, this ability to utilize gestures may be enhanced in bilingual children, who may be more sensitive to such cues due to their unique language experience. We asked monolingual and bilingual…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Monolingualism, Bilingual Students, Adults
Vida, Mark D.; Maurer, Daphne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Adults use eye contact as a cue to the mental and emotional states of others. Here, we examined developmental changes in the ability to discriminate between eye contact and averted gaze. Children (6-, 8-, 10-, and 14-year-olds) and adults (n=18/age) viewed photographs of a model fixating the center of a camera lens and a series of positions to the…
Descriptors: Photography, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Children
Winder, Breanna M.; Wozniak, Robert H.; Parladé, Meaghan V.; Iverson, Jana M. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Communication spontaneously initiated by infants at heightened risk (HR; n = 15) for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is compared with that in low-risk (LR; n = 15) infants at 13 and 18 months of age. Infants were observed longitudinally during naturalistic in-home interaction and semistructured play with caregivers. At both ages, HR infants…
Descriptors: Communication Skills, Infants, At Risk Persons, Autism
Barthomeuf, Laetitia; Droit-Volet, Sylvie; Rousset, Sylvie – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2012
The aim of this study was to determine whether or not pleasure, neutrality, and disgust expressed by eaters in photographs could affect the desire to eat food products to a greater extent in children than in adults. Children of 5 and 8 years of age, as well as adults, were presented with photographs of liked and disliked foods. These foods were…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Young Children, Developmental Psychology, Adults