Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Child Psychology | 10 |
Comparative Analysis | 10 |
Task Analysis | 10 |
Children | 6 |
Adults | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 5 |
Experimental Psychology | 5 |
Models | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Child Development | 3 |
Correlation | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 6 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Developmental Psychology | 1 |
Journal of Abnormal Child… | 1 |
Journal of Child Psychology… | 1 |
Author
Atance, Cristina M. | 1 |
Clawson, Ann | 1 |
Clayson, Peter E. | 1 |
Clegg, Jennifer M. | 1 |
Cole, David A. | 1 |
Defever, Emmy | 1 |
Feigenson, Lisa | 1 |
Fisher, Anna V. | 1 |
Franz, Volker H. | 1 |
Gebuis, Titia | 1 |
Jovanovic, Bianca | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 5 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
United States | 1 |
Vanuatu | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Clegg, Jennifer M.; Legare, Cristine H. – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Recent research with Western populations has demonstrated that children use imitation flexibly to engage in both instrumental and conventional learning. Evidence for children's imitative flexibility in non-Western populations is limited, however, and has only assessed imitation of instrumental tasks. This study (N = 142, 6- to 8-year-olds)…
Descriptors: Imitation, Cross Cultural Studies, Comparative Analysis, Children
Kenward, Ben – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Over-imitation, which is common in children, is the imitation of elements of an action sequence that are clearly unnecessary for reaching the final goal. A variety of cognitive mechanisms have been proposed to explain this phenomenon. Here, 48 3- and 5-year-olds together with a puppet observed an adult demonstrate instrumental tasks that included…
Descriptors: Conferences (Gatherings), Puppetry, Imitation, Critical Thinking
Sloutsky, Vladimir M.; Fisher, Anna V. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Linguistic labels affect inductive generalization; however, the mechanism underlying these effects remains unclear. According to one similarity-based model, SINC (similarity, induction, naming, and categorization), early in development labels are features of objects contributing to the overall similarity of compared entities, with early induction…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Infants, Logical Thinking, Adults
Schum, Nina; Franz, Volker H.; Jovanovic, Bianca; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We investigated whether 6- and 7-year-olds and 9- and 10-year-olds, as well as adults, process object dimensions independent of or in interaction with one another in a perception and action task by adapting Ganel and Goodale's method for testing adults ("Nature", 2003, Vol. 426, pp. 664-667). In addition, we aimed to confirm Ganel and Goodale's…
Descriptors: Evidence, Handicrafts, Visual Perception, Interaction
Weitlauf, Amy S.; Cole, David A. – Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 2012
Attributional style models of depression in adults (Abramson et al. 1989, 1978) have been adapted for use with children; however, most applications do not consider that children's understanding of causal relations may be qualitatively different from that of adults. If children's causal attributions depend on children's level of cognitive…
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Depression (Psychology), Cognitive Development, Models
Moher, Mariko; Tuerk, Arin S.; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Although working memory has a highly constrained capacity limit of three or four items, both adults and toddlers can increase the total amount of stored information by "chunking" object representations in memory. To examine the developmental origins of chunking, we used a violation-of-expectation procedure to ask whether 7-month-old infants, whose…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Infants, Short Term Memory
Larson, Michael J.; South, Mikle; Clayson, Peter E.; Clawson, Ann – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2012
Background: Youth diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) often show deficits in cognitive control processes, potentially contributing to characteristic difficulties monitoring and regulating behavior. Modification of performance following conflict can be measured by examining conflict adaptation, the adjustment of cognitive resources based…
Descriptors: Autism, Conflict, Disabilities, Cognitive Processes
Nittrouer, Susan; Shune, Samantha; Lowenstein, Joanna H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Although children with language impairments, including those associated with reading, usually demonstrate deficits in phonological processing, there is minimal agreement as to the source of those deficits. This study examined two problems hypothesized to be possible sources: either poor auditory sensitivity to speech-relevant acoustic properties,…
Descriptors: Cues, Language Impairments, Phonological Awareness, Word Recognition
Metcalf, Jennifer L.; Atance, Cristina M. – Cognitive Development, 2011
Using a new paradigm for measuring children's saving behaviors involving two marble games differing in desirability, we assessed whether 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds saved marbles for future use, saved increasingly on a second trial, saved increasingly with age, and were sensitive to the relative value of future rewards. We also assessed whether…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Models, Rewards, Cognitive Development
Defever, Emmy; Sasanguie, Delphine; Gebuis, Titia; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
How people process and represent magnitude has often been studied using number comparison tasks. From the results of these tasks, a comparison distance effect (CDE) is generated, showing that it is easier to discriminate two numbers that are numerically further apart (e.g., 2 and 8) compared with numerically closer numbers (e.g., 6 and 8).…
Descriptors: Models, Mathematics Tests, Kindergarten, Grade 6