Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 14 |
Descriptor
Computation | 16 |
Numbers | 11 |
Number Concepts | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Numeracy | 5 |
Children | 4 |
Logical Thinking | 4 |
Inferences | 3 |
Thinking Skills | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Animals | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 16 |
Author
Carey, Susan | 3 |
Asmuth, Jennifer | 2 |
Bloomfield, Amber | 2 |
Henik, Avishai | 2 |
Kadosh, Roi Cohen | 2 |
Rips, Lance J. | 2 |
Tzelgov, Joseph | 2 |
Antonucci, Cristina | 1 |
Bethune, Narelle E. | 1 |
Canobi, Katherine H. | 1 |
Dehaene, Stanislas | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Opinion Papers | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Audience
Location
Nicaragua | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pepperberg, Irene M.; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2012
A Grey parrot ("Psittacus erithacus") had previously been taught to use English count words ("one" through "sih" [six]) to label sets of one to six individual items (Pepperberg, 1994). He had also been taught to use the same count words to label the Arabic numerals 1 through 6. Without training, he inferred the relationship between the Arabic…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, Number Concepts, Animals, Computation
Piantadosi, Steven T.; Tenenbaum, Joshua B.; Goodman, Noah D. – Cognition, 2012
In acquiring number words, children exhibit a qualitative leap in which they transition from understanding a few number words, to possessing a rich system of interrelated numerical concepts. We present a computational framework for understanding this inductive leap as the consequence of statistical inference over a sufficiently powerful…
Descriptors: Statistical Inference, Number Concepts, Models, Computation
Lu, Aitao; Hodges, Bert; Zhang, Jijia; Zhang, John X. – Cognition, 2009
Time perception has long been known to be affected by numerical representations. Recent studies further demonstrate that when participants estimate the duration of Arabic numbers, number magnitude, though task-irrelevant, biases duration judgment to produce underestimation for smaller numbers and overestimation for larger numbers. Such effects…
Descriptors: Numbers, Computation, Context Effect, Cognitive Processes
Flaherty, Molly; Senghas, Ann – Cognition, 2011
What abilities are entailed in being numerate? Certainly, one is the ability to hold the exact quantity of a set in mind, even as it changes, and even after its members can no longer be perceived. Is counting language necessary to track and reproduce exact quantities? Previous work with speakers of languages that lack number words involved…
Descriptors: Deafness, Computation, Sign Language, Adults
Sarnecka, Barbara W.; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2008
This study compared 2- to 4-year-olds who understand how counting works ("cardinal-principle-knowers") to those who do not ("subset-knowers"), in order to better characterize the knowledge itself. New results are that (1) Many children answer the question "how many" with the last word used in counting, despite not understanding how counting works;…
Descriptors: Computation, Numbers, Children
Izard, Veronique; Dehaene, Stanislas – Cognition, 2008
Human adults are thought to possess two dissociable systems to represent numbers: an approximate quantity system akin to a mental number line, and a verbal system capable of representing numbers exactly. Here, we study the interface between these two systems using an estimation task. Observers were asked to estimate the approximate numerosity of…
Descriptors: Computation, Numbers, Stimuli, Concept Mapping
Canobi, Katherine H.; Bethune, Narelle E. – Cognition, 2008
Three studies addressed children's arithmetic. First, 50 3- to 5-year-olds judged physical demonstrations of addition, subtraction and inversion, with and without number words. Second, 20 3- to 4-year-olds made equivalence judgments of additions and subtractions. Third, 60 4- to 6-year-olds solved addition, subtraction and inversion problems that…
Descriptors: Numbers, Computation, Subtraction, Arithmetic
Kadosh, Roi Cohen; Tzelgov, Joseph; Henik, Avishai – Cognition, 2008
Verguts and Van Opstal [Verguts, T., & Van Opstal, F. (2008). A colorful walk, but is it on the mental number line? Reply to Cohen Kadosh, Tzelgov, and Henik, Cognition, 106, 558-563] cleverly explained the results of Cohen Kadosh, Tzelgov, and Henik [Cohen Kadosh, R., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A. (2008). A synesthetic walk on the mental number line:…
Descriptors: Measurement Techniques, Computation, Numbers, Cognitive Processes
Domahs, Frank; Moeller, Korbinian; Huber, Stefan; Willmes, Klaus; Nuerk, Hans-Christoph – Cognition, 2010
In recent years, a strong functional relationship between finger counting and number processing has been suggested. Developmental studies have shown specific effects of the structure of the individual finger counting system on arithmetic abilities. Moreover, the orientation of the mental quantity representation ("number line") seems to be…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Computation, German, Mathematics Instruction
Kadosh, Roi Cohen; Tzelgov, Joseph; Henik, Avishai – Cognition, 2008
Are small and large numbers represented similarly or differently on the mental number line? The size effect was used to argue that numbers are represented differently. However, recently it has been argued that the size effect is due to the comparison task and is not derived from the mental number line per se. Namely, it is due to the way that the…
Descriptors: Measurement Techniques, Numbers, Computation, Effect Size
Le Corre, Mathieu; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2007
Since the publication of [Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). "The child's understanding of number." Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.] seminal work on the development of verbal counting as a representation of number, the nature of the ontogenetic sources of the verbal counting principles has been intensely debated. The present…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Number Concepts, Computation, Children
Rips, Lance J.; Asmuth, Jennifer; Bloomfield, Amber – Cognition, 2008
According to one theory about how children learn the meaning of the words for the positive integers, they first learn that "one," "two," and "three" stand for appropriately sized sets. They then conclude by inductive inference that the next numeral in the count sequence denotes the size of sets containing one more object than the size denoted by…
Descriptors: Learning Strategies, Logical Thinking, Number Concepts, Inferences
Rips, Lance J.; Asmuth, Jennifer; Bloomfield, Amber – Cognition, 2006
According to one theory about how children learn the concept of natural numbers, they first determine that "one", "two", and "three" denote the size of sets containing the relevant number of items. They then make the following inductive inference (the Bootstrap): The next number word in the counting series denotes the size of the sets you get by…
Descriptors: Numbers, Number Concepts, Inferences, Computation
Lipton, Jennifer S.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2006
Although children take over a year to learn the meanings of the first three number words, they eventually master the logic of counting and the meanings of all the words in their count list. Here, we ask whether children's knowledge applies to number words beyond those they have mastered: Does a child who can only count to 20 infer that number…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Numbers, Semantics, Emergent Literacy
Flombaum, Jonathan I.; Junge, Justin A.; Hauser, Marc D. – Cognition, 2005
Mathematics is a uniquely human capacity. Studies of animals and human infants reveal, however, that this capacity builds on language-independent mechanisms for quantifying small numbers ([less than] 4) precisely and large numbers approximately. It is unclear whether animals and human infants can spontaneously tap mechanisms for quantifying large…
Descriptors: Numbers, Animals, Infants, Arithmetic
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2